2007
DOI: 10.1348/026151006x103654
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Preadolescents' categorization of gender and ethnicity at the subgroup level in memory

Abstract: The statement matching paradigm was used to examine how 10‐ to 13‐year‐olds categorized adults when both ethnicity and gender varied across targets. Forty‐seven children watched a PowerPoint presentation of a conversation involving two Black men, two Black women, two White men and two White women. Each slide displayed the speaker's picture, name and the statement made. The children then were asked to determine who made each statement. The pattern of errors in their assignments was examined to assess how they s… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
11
0

Year Published

2008
2008
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 13 publications
(12 citation statements)
references
References 41 publications
1
11
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Some children shared their source of knowledge, indicating that they had learned about the race or gender of the presidents via books, posters, dictionaries, encyclopedias, rulers, parents, television, and currency. “Have you seen the money?” one observant girl asked, “Only boy pictures are on the money.” These data are consistent with other work indicating that children attend to the race and gender of individuals (Susskind, 2007) and are aware of status differences related to race and gender early in development (Liben et al, 2001). Based on Brown and Bigler's (2005) model of children's perceptions of discrimination, we expected that children who are members of groups that have been excluded from the role of the president would be especially knowledgeable about the race and gender of U.S. presidents.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 91%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Some children shared their source of knowledge, indicating that they had learned about the race or gender of the presidents via books, posters, dictionaries, encyclopedias, rulers, parents, television, and currency. “Have you seen the money?” one observant girl asked, “Only boy pictures are on the money.” These data are consistent with other work indicating that children attend to the race and gender of individuals (Susskind, 2007) and are aware of status differences related to race and gender early in development (Liben et al, 2001). Based on Brown and Bigler's (2005) model of children's perceptions of discrimination, we expected that children who are members of groups that have been excluded from the role of the president would be especially knowledgeable about the race and gender of U.S. presidents.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 91%
“…Nonetheless, children might acquire such knowledge through observation and constructive processes. Previous work (Bigler & Liben, 2006, 2007; Susskind, 2007) suggests that children routinely attend to race and gender because these characteristics are (1) readily perceptually discriminable, (2) frequently labeled (especially true of gender, e.g., “Good morning, boys and girls.”), and (3) the bases of the sorting of individuals in the culture (especially true of race, e.g., residential segregation). Thus, we predicted that even young children would be knowledgeable about the links among gender, race, and the U.S. presidency.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It also, like race, appeared to be a default dimension of person perception and categorization in previous research (Beauvais & Spence, 1987;Cabecinhas & Amâncio, 1999;Frable & Bem, 1985;Jackson & Hymes, 1985;Miller, 1986;Lorenzi-Cioldi, 1993;Sani, Bennett, & Soutar, 2005;Stangor et al, 1992;Susskind, 2007;Taylor et al, 1978;Taylor & Falcone, 1982;Van Twyver & Van Knippenberg, 1998). Unlike race, however, sex is biologically real, has been present since the advent of sexual reproduction, and organizes behavior in all known primate societies, including all human societies (Sidanius & Pratto, 1999;Smuts et al, 1987;Sugiyama, 2005).…”
mentioning
confidence: 58%
“…This conclusion was based on decades of unsuccessful attempts to inhibit or reduce people's implicit categorization of others by race. These included: priming race, priming a dimension that cross-cuts race, manipulating contextual relevance (i.e., showing people discussing race relations or some other topic), and explicit instructions to either attend or not attend to race (e.g., Bennett & Sani, 2003;Hewstone, Hantzi, & Johnson, 1991;Stangor, Lynch, Duan, & Glass, 1992;Susskind, 2007;Taylor, Fiske, Etcoff, & Ruderman, 1978).…”
Section: Racial Categorization As a Byproduct Of The Alliance Detectimentioning
confidence: 99%
“…First, children routinely attend to race because it is perceptually discriminable and serves as a basis for the sorting of individuals in the culture (e.g., via de facto segregation; Bigler & Liben, 2006). Second, children are aware of race as a social category and can understand and apply racial labels in early childhood (Aboud, 1988;Susskind, 2007). Thus, we expected our elementaryschool-aged participants to identify correctly the racial group memberships of both Obama and McCain.…”
Section: Children's Knowledge Of Race and Politicsmentioning
confidence: 99%