The Oxford Handbook of Polysynthesis 2017
DOI: 10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199683208.013.25
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Polysynthesis in the Acquisition of Inuit Languages

Abstract: In this chapter, I begin by briefly outlining the structure of Inuit (Eskimo) languages and the challenges they present for child language development. In the bulk of the chapter, I review the existing literature on the first language, impaired, and bilingual acquisition of Inuit languages (i.e. Inuktitut and West Greenlandic) from ages 1 through 16 years. Structures covered include nursery vocabulary, word-internal derivational morphology, verbal and nominal inflectional morphology, other complex morphology, … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1

Citation Types

0
9
0

Year Published

2018
2018
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
4
3
2

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 28 publications
(9 citation statements)
references
References 199 publications
0
9
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Virtually all Inuit children in eastern Canada learn Inuktitut as their native language. We have a reasonable understanding of the trajectory of many aspects of language acquisition in typically-developing Inuktitut-speaking children as a result of research conducted over the past three decades (e.g., Allen, 1996, 1998, 2000, 2013, in press; Allen & Crago, 1996; Allen, Crago, & Pesco, 2006; Crago, 1988; Crago & Allen, 1997, 1998, 2001; Crago, Annahatak, Doehring, & Allen, 1991; Dorais & Sammons, 2002; Fortescue & Lennert Olsen, 1992; Parkinson, 1999; Swift, 2004; Wilman, 1988; Wright, Taylor, & Macarthur, 2000). However, as noted in a 2010 report published by Speech-Language and Audiology Canada (SAC, 2010), there is a critical lack of tools that can be used by speech-language pathologists to assess language ability and language difficulties in children who speak Inuktitut or other indigenous languages.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Virtually all Inuit children in eastern Canada learn Inuktitut as their native language. We have a reasonable understanding of the trajectory of many aspects of language acquisition in typically-developing Inuktitut-speaking children as a result of research conducted over the past three decades (e.g., Allen, 1996, 1998, 2000, 2013, in press; Allen & Crago, 1996; Allen, Crago, & Pesco, 2006; Crago, 1988; Crago & Allen, 1997, 1998, 2001; Crago, Annahatak, Doehring, & Allen, 1991; Dorais & Sammons, 2002; Fortescue & Lennert Olsen, 1992; Parkinson, 1999; Swift, 2004; Wilman, 1988; Wright, Taylor, & Macarthur, 2000). However, as noted in a 2010 report published by Speech-Language and Audiology Canada (SAC, 2010), there is a critical lack of tools that can be used by speech-language pathologists to assess language ability and language difficulties in children who speak Inuktitut or other indigenous languages.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In addition, virtually all children learn Inuktitut at home as their native language. Numerous studies attest to the fluent acquisition of Inuktitut by preschool Inuit children in Nunavik (e.g., Allen, 1996, in press; Crago & Allen, 1998; Swift, 2004), as well as to the strong Inuktitut abilities of school-aged Inuit children in the early grades (e.g., Allen et al, 2006; Crago et al, 1991; Wright et al, 2000).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…structural features allow for the possibility of different degrees of consistency of the polysynthetic type. 17 The study of acquisition of polysynthetic structures is its infancy; see Allen (2017), Forshaw et al (2017), and Stoll, Mazara, and Bickel (2017) for three recent case studies and the references therein. Although much more work needs to be done here, the results suggest that children acquire richer and more complex morphologies differently from how they acquire, say, English morphology.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, children acquiring Inuit languages productively use grammatical marking for possession around two years of age, earlier than English-acquiring children (Allen, 2017;Fortescue & Olsen, 1992). This early emergence of inflection is likely a function of inflection-heavy language-specific characteristics as well as adult input (Allen, 2017). However, data from other polysynthetic languages more closely resemble cross-linguistic trends in the acquisition of possessive marking.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%