2013
DOI: 10.1007/s10683-013-9386-4
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Playing the trust game with other people’s money

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

1
9
0
2

Year Published

2017
2017
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
5
3

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 20 publications
(12 citation statements)
references
References 36 publications
1
9
0
2
Order By: Relevance
“…5. This flat payment to the sender/intermediary also aligns with the approch used by Kvaløy and Luzuriaga (2014) and Maximiano et al (2013).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 53%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…5. This flat payment to the sender/intermediary also aligns with the approch used by Kvaløy and Luzuriaga (2014) and Maximiano et al (2013).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 53%
“…In many situations where decision-making is delegated (including the examples noted earlier), trust is as important as it is in bilateral interactions. Recently, Maximiano, Sloof, and Sonnemans (2013) and Kvaløy and Luzuriaga (2014) have investigated the impact of delegated decision-making on trust and reciprocity. Kvaløy and Luzuriaga used a trust game set-up; while Maximiano et al used a gift-exchange game to study worker effort (reciprocity) under different management scenarios.…”
Section: Literaturementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Using the same investment task, Pollmann, Potters, and Trautmann (2014) found that risk-taking increased whereas Eriksen and Kvaløy (2010) found that risk-taking decreased when making decisions for others. Using a similar task, Kvaløy, Eriksen, and Luzuriaga (2014) identified that risk-averse participants took more risks on behalf of another person, whereas risk-seeking participants took fewer risks. These inconsistencies do not seem to be attributable to the type of task used.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Studies which found that risk aversion decreases in surrogate decisions, in the psychology literature, used hypothetical decisions for a friend ( Beisswanger et al, 2003 ; Stone & Allgaier, 2008 ; Stone et al, 2013 ; Wray & Stone, 2005 ) or for a stranger ( Ziegler & Tunney, 2015 ), and in the economic literature used real decisions for a stranger ( Chakravarty et al, 2011 ; Kvaløy et al, 2014 ; Mengarelli et al, 2014 ; Polman, 2012 ; Pollmann et al., 2014 ). Studies which found that risk aversion increases in surrogate decisions used real decisions for a stranger ( Kvaløy et al, 2014 ), for the participant and a stranger ( Pahlke, Strasser, & Vieider, 2015 ), or for a group ( Reynolds et al, 2011 ). Studies which found an absence of difference used hypothetical decisions for a friend ( Benjamin & Robbins, 2007 ; Stone et al, 2002 ) or real decisions for a stranger ( Stone et al, 2002 ).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Bigoni et al 2013;Bauernschuster, Falck, and Große 2013) attempt to split the trustworthiness or trusting decision among two players, but retain a fixed multiplier value and sequential play between the three players. Recent research has investigated the implications of playing the trust game, either on behalf of others ("clients"; Kvaløy and Luzuriaga 2014) or with other people's money (Jones 2013). When studied in the context of a multi-level trust game, such 'brokering' could enable an understanding of the decisions individuals made when not handling their own resources.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%