2004
DOI: 10.1016/j.jml.2004.07.001
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Phonological phrase boundaries constrain lexical access I. Adult data

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2

Citation Types

6
122
1
4

Year Published

2006
2006
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
5
2
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 148 publications
(134 citation statements)
references
References 47 publications
6
122
1
4
Order By: Relevance
“…Rather, the language-specific pronunciation norms along this dimension were maintained in clear and conversational speech. While accumulated production and perception results from previous studies showed that fine grained phonetic detail of segmental duration encodes information from various linguistic levels and in turn governs speech comprehension ͑Cho et Cutler and Otake, 1994;Fougeron, 1999;Christophe et al, 2004͒, these data suggest that the same relative durations for these effects should be maintained across changes in speaking styles and rates. In other words, in order for listeners to successfully interpret the speech signal and derive information about linguistic structure ͑lexical access, segment identification, prosodic structure, etc.͒, they may rely on local timing relations, which can be measured as durational ratios.…”
Section: Summary and Discussionmentioning
confidence: 74%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Rather, the language-specific pronunciation norms along this dimension were maintained in clear and conversational speech. While accumulated production and perception results from previous studies showed that fine grained phonetic detail of segmental duration encodes information from various linguistic levels and in turn governs speech comprehension ͑Cho et Cutler and Otake, 1994;Fougeron, 1999;Christophe et al, 2004͒, these data suggest that the same relative durations for these effects should be maintained across changes in speaking styles and rates. In other words, in order for listeners to successfully interpret the speech signal and derive information about linguistic structure ͑lexical access, segment identification, prosodic structure, etc.͒, they may rely on local timing relations, which can be measured as durational ratios.…”
Section: Summary and Discussionmentioning
confidence: 74%
“…Moreover, informal observations showed an increase in the number of intermediate ͑ip͒ and IPs within PPs as well, i.e., a number of smaller PPs that did not coincide with the pauses were produced as well. These phrases are often marked by intonation patterns ͑F0͒, phrase-initial and -final lengthening and reduced coarticula-tion between phonemes across boundaries ͑Wightman et Keating et al, 2003;Byrd et al, 2000͒. There is evidence that the acoustic correlates of these phrases, as well as of word-level stress, aid listeners in lexical segmentation, i.e., in finding word boundaries and in resolving lexical competition ͑Cho et Cutler and Otake, 1994;Fougeron, 1999;Christophe et al, 2004͒. Christophe et al ͑2004͒ found that monosyllabic words were accessed faster in a two-word sequence when the two words belonged to two separate phonological phrases than when they belonged to the same phrase.…”
Section: Summary and Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Recently, in a study in French (Christophe, Peperkamp, Pallier, Block, & Mehler, 2004), listeners were presented with sentences containing a local lexical ambiguity. For example, the phrase chat grincheux (grumpy cat) contains the word chagrin (sorrow).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Listeners can use the acoustic correlates of prosodic structure to decode those structures, facilitating segmentation of words at prosodic boundaries (e.g., Christophe, Peperkamp, Pallier, Block, & Mehler, 2004). For instance, word-and phrase-final lengthening appears to facilitate segmentation of words not only in continuous native-language speech (Kim & Cho, 2009;Salverda, Dahan, & McQueen, 2003), but also in artificial-language speech streams (Bagou, Fougeron, & Frauenfelder, 2002;Kim, Broersma, & Cho, 2012;Saffran, Newport, & Aslin, 1996;Tyler & Cutler, 2009).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…We therefore do not know whether word-initial stops embedded in a phrase are indeed produced with shorter VOTs than wordmedial stops in Dutch. But we do know that phrase-boundary cues (which necessarily also occur at word boundaries) facilitate lexical segmentation relative to phrase-internal word-boundary cues (e.g., Cho et al, 2007;Christophe et al, 2004;Kim & Cho, 2009). Given that shortened VOTs mark Dutch phrase boundaries, Dutch listeners may make use of these cues when segmenting novel lexical sequences that occur in an artificial language.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%