2008
DOI: 10.1121/1.2990712
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Temporal organization of English clear and conversational speech

Abstract: This study investigated the effect of hyperarticulated, intelligibility-enhancing clear speech on temporal characteristics as reflected in number, durations, and variability of consonant and vowel intervals in sentence-and paragraph-length utterances. The results of sentence-in-noise listening tests showed a consistent clear speech intelligibility gain across the utterances of varying complexity indicating that the talkers successfully maintained clear speech articulatory modifications throughout longer stretc… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

1
34
0

Year Published

2010
2010
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
9
1

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 53 publications
(37 citation statements)
references
References 57 publications
1
34
0
Order By: Relevance
“…This can be explained by differences between the two speech styles. In laboratory speech, the sentences were clearly and carefully pronounced by one speaker only, whereas in casual speech, the sentences contained more noise and were uttered by multiple speakers (e.g., Smiljanic & Bradlow, 2008a;Bradlow, Nygaard, & Pisoni, 1999). *** Table 2 about here *** Casual speech for the first second after target word onset.…”
Section: Reduced Speech and Lexical Competitionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This can be explained by differences between the two speech styles. In laboratory speech, the sentences were clearly and carefully pronounced by one speaker only, whereas in casual speech, the sentences contained more noise and were uttered by multiple speakers (e.g., Smiljanic & Bradlow, 2008a;Bradlow, Nygaard, & Pisoni, 1999). *** Table 2 about here *** Casual speech for the first second after target word onset.…”
Section: Reduced Speech and Lexical Competitionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Common instructions for eliciting clear speech include "Speak clearly" (Ferguson, 2004;Ferguson & KewleyPort, 2007), "Hyperarticulate" (Dromey, 2000;Moon & Lindblom, 1994), "Speak to someone with a hearing impairment" (Bradlow et al, 2003), "Speak with nonnative speakers" (Smiljanic & Bradlow, 2008), and "Speak to someone who has difficulty understanding you" (Goberman & Elmer, 2005;Rosen et al, 2011). Other studies have elicited clear speech through an imitation task or training paradigm (Beukelman, Fager, Ullman, Hanson, & Logemann, 2002;Krause & Braida, 2004), by providing a grade or reward system (Perkell, Zandipour, Matthies, & Lane, 2002), or by asking for a repetition (Maniwa et al, 2009;Oviatt, MacEachern, & Levow, 1998).…”
Section: Clear Speech Instructionsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Talkers speaking clearly speak more slowly (Picheny et al, 1986;Bradlow et al, 2003;Smiljanic and Bradlow, 2005;Smiljanic and Bradlow, 2008;Ferguson et al, 2010;Rosen et al, 2011;Lam et al, 2012) and more loudly (Picheny et al, 1986;Ferguson et al, 2010;Hazan and Baker, 2011; 2 Lam et al, 2012) and with a higher voice pitch (Bradlow et al, 2003;Hazan and Baker, 2011). Talkers also use a more variable voice pitch in clear speech than in conversational speech (Picheny et al, 1986;Bradlow et al, 2003;Hazan and Baker, 2011).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%