2016
DOI: 10.1111/synt.12120
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Phasal Syntax = Cyclic Phonology?

Abstract: This paper addresses three central questions in the phonology-syntax interface: What does phonology know about syntax? Does phrasal phonology "know" about syntax directly or indirectly (i.e., mediated by prosodic constituents such as Intonation Phrase)? When does the phonology-syntax interaction take place? Most current phase-based theories of the interface assume a strict cyclic model of derivation, in which the output of each spell-out domain directly feeds the phonology. We argue instead for an indirect mod… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

0
27
0
3

Year Published

2017
2017
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

1
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 28 publications
(30 citation statements)
references
References 54 publications
(184 reference statements)
0
27
0
3
Order By: Relevance
“…However, because it retains a constraint-based view of prosodic structure building, CBP avoids the pitfalls associated with direct reference theories of prosodic structure, which assume that prosodic domains are always identical to syntactic domains (see Cheng and Downing 2016 for discussion).…”
Section: Predictions Of Cophonologies By Phasementioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, because it retains a constraint-based view of prosodic structure building, CBP avoids the pitfalls associated with direct reference theories of prosodic structure, which assume that prosodic domains are always identical to syntactic domains (see Cheng and Downing 2016 for discussion).…”
Section: Predictions Of Cophonologies By Phasementioning
confidence: 99%
“…But if new data revealed a more general syntactic configuration underlying the H-tones in Samoan, e.g., if every adjoined phrase were marked with an H-, that could also be represented in a minimalist grammar, composed into the syntax; the proposal here does not hang on a particular, precise account of the syntactic conditioning of H-. It could also be informative to formalize interface theories that refer to syntactic phases, e.g., see Ahn (2016b); Dobashi (2004); Kratzer and Selkirk (2007);Dobashi (2009);Cheng and Downing (2016).…”
Section: Strategic Principle 2: Testing Hypotheses With Computationalmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…∅-somele chi-búuku 'he read a book' c. ∅-somele chi-buku íchi 'he read this book' Based on these patterns, Kisseberth (in a number of works), along with Selkirk (1986) and Truckenbrodt (1995Truckenbrodt ( , 1999, has argued that a prosodic phrase break follows every lexical XP; more examples illustrating this point are given in (26) As we can see, there is always a phrase break separating the subject and the verb, and there is always a phrase break separating post-verbal complements. As Cheng & Downing (2016), following others, show, these are the breaks predicted by a constraint aligning prosodic phrase edges with lexical XP edges:…”
Section: The Problem Of Chimwiinimentioning
confidence: 83%
“…As Cheng & Downing (2016) point out, it is quite common in Bantu languages for modified nouns to have different phrasing properties from unmodified ones. Besides the examples cited here, the effect of modifiers on phrasing in Tsonga has received attention, as it forms a central case study in Selkirk's (2011) handbook chapter.…”
Section: The Problem Of Modifiersmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation