1997
DOI: 10.2466/pms.84.1.131-138
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Performance on the Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised for Patients With Mild Traumatic Brain Injury and Mild Dementia

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
4
0

Year Published

2002
2002
2012
2012

Publication Types

Select...
3

Relationship

0
3

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 3 publications
(4 citation statements)
references
References 9 publications
0
4
0
Order By: Relevance
“…With regard to AD, previous research (Brooker, 1997) has shown that AD patients score lower than healthy adults on both verbal memory and visual memory as measured by the WMS-R. To our knowledge there is no previous research studying the effects of AD on prospective memory or memory of faces. Therefore, it is not clear whether an AD condition affects all aspects of memory performance and metamemory reports.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 91%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…With regard to AD, previous research (Brooker, 1997) has shown that AD patients score lower than healthy adults on both verbal memory and visual memory as measured by the WMS-R. To our knowledge there is no previous research studying the effects of AD on prospective memory or memory of faces. Therefore, it is not clear whether an AD condition affects all aspects of memory performance and metamemory reports.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 91%
“…Therefore, it is essential to investigate the relationships between the two objective memory tests and the EMQ in a broader range of age groups. It is also interesting to test patients with Alzheimer's disease (AD) as a comparison group to healthy adults, since in this group of patients we know that their objective performance is lower than that of healthy adults (Brooker, 1997; Robinson-Whelen & Storandt, 1992). It is expected that, unlike the Koltai et al (1996) study, the relationships between the WMS, the RBMT, and the EMQ will differ from the relationships in the healthy group.…”
Section: Aims Of the Studymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Both K-ABC subtests have been found to correlate highly with other tests of working memory, for example, the WISC-R Arithmetic (.65) and Digit Span (.63) tests (e.g., Boivin et al, 2010;Meesters, Van Gastel, Ghys, & Merckelbach, 1998), and they have been proven to be sensitive to revealing the cognitive sequelae of learning disability (e.g., ADHD, MD) and neuropathology (e.g., mild traumatic brain injury; Aunola, Leskinen, Lerkkanen, & Nurmi, 2004;Fox & Fox, 2001;Frencham, Fox, & Maybery, 2003;Knauss, Stavrinos, Pendleton, de Jong, & Schwebel, 2009;Shum, Neulinger, O'Callaghan, & Mohay, 2008;Smyth & Pendleton, 1989). HM may be even more sensitive than other, more "standard" tests (e.g., the Knox test) to predict working memory deficits in these populations (as suggested by e.g., Barry & Riley, 1987;Brooker, 1997;Fox & Fox, 2001;Frencham et al, 2003). Also, the value of the K-ABC subtests has been repeatedly demonstrated while finding support for the role of working memory in time estimation or TR in, for example, healthy individuals (e.g., Brocki & Bohlin, 2004;Zélanti & Droit-Volet, 2011).…”
Section: Time Reproduction Task (Tr)mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Differences in short-term memory performance for movement sequences compared to verbal and visuospatial sequences have also been noted in the neuropsychological literature. As mentioned, individuals with TBI have been found to perform poorly on the KHMT, yet they are unimpaired on digit span and visual span tasks from the Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised (WMS-R; Wechsler, 1987); tests argued to tap short-term verbal and visuospatial memory (Barry & Riley, 1987;Brooker, 1997;Fox et al, 1993;Fox & Fox, 2001). One interpretation of these findings is that components of short-term memory are differentially affected following TBI.…”
Section: Neuropsychological Assessment and Movement Memory Deficitsmentioning
confidence: 99%