2008
DOI: 10.3758/pp.70.6.1117
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Perceptual learning and the visual control of braking

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

3
44
1

Year Published

2009
2009
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 48 publications
(48 citation statements)
references
References 34 publications
3
44
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Overall, a much more powerful explanation for the present observations can be obtained from response threshold models of brake timing, which postulate that drivers initiate braking once some visual cue, for example , reaches a certain threshold value (Lee, 1976;Kiefer et al, 2003Kiefer et al, , 2005Flach et al, 2004;Fajen, 2005Fajen, , 2008Wada et al, 2009;Treiber et al, 2013). Previously, such models have mainly been studied in the context of routine driving, but the results presented here, for example in Figure 6, suggest that a threshold-based model also works very well for describing behavior in surprise rear-end emergencies: In eyes-on-threat events, few drivers responded before visual looming reached s (or rad s, or m s ), and most drivers responded within a second after reaching this threshold 3 .…”
Section: Deceleration Timing From Looming Thresholds?mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Overall, a much more powerful explanation for the present observations can be obtained from response threshold models of brake timing, which postulate that drivers initiate braking once some visual cue, for example , reaches a certain threshold value (Lee, 1976;Kiefer et al, 2003Kiefer et al, , 2005Flach et al, 2004;Fajen, 2005Fajen, , 2008Wada et al, 2009;Treiber et al, 2013). Previously, such models have mainly been studied in the context of routine driving, but the results presented here, for example in Figure 6, suggest that a threshold-based model also works very well for describing behavior in surprise rear-end emergencies: In eyes-on-threat events, few drivers responded before visual looming reached s (or rad s, or m s ), and most drivers responded within a second after reaching this threshold 3 .…”
Section: Deceleration Timing From Looming Thresholds?mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Inverse tau is a visually available estimate of inverse TTC (Lee, 1976), and thus increases as the potential collision draws nearer. Furthermore, it has often been suggested that inverse tau and similar quantities play an important role in determining drivers' responses to obstacles and collision threats (Lee, 1976;Kiefer et al, 2003Kiefer et al, , 2005Fajen, 2005Fajen, , 2008Kondoh et al, 2008Kondoh et al, , 2014Jurecki & Stanczyk, 2009, 2014.…”
Section: Analyses Of Kinematics-dependencementioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…One interpretation of such changes is as improvements in perception of aVordances (see Ramenzoni et al 2008Ramenzoni et al , 2010StoVregen et al 2009;Weast et al 2011). For example, perception of aVordances for passing through an opening more closely approximates actual ability following practice performing this behavior (Franchak et al 2010), and the distance between actual and intended stopping location decreases with practice in a simulated braking task (Fajen 2008;Fajen and Devany 2006). Moreover, such changes in perception often transfer to perception of aVordances for unpracticed (but related) behaviors.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 88%
“…Perceivers often start with the detection of variables that relate ambiguously to the environmental property, but move to the exploitation of specifying information after feedback; that is, they converge on the detection of informational variables that relate one-toone to the to-be-perceived property. The recent upsurge in studies of perceptual learning has demonstrated that this process occurs in many tasks: the visual perception of the pulling force of a stick figure (e.g., Michaels & de Vries, 1998), the visual perception of the relative mass of colliding balls (e.g., Jacobs, Michaels, & Runeson, 2000;Jacobs, Runeson, & Michaels, 2001;Runeson & Andersson, 2007;Runeson, Juslin, & Olsson, 2000), height and length perception by dynamic touch (Michaels, Arzamarski, Isenhower, & Jacobs, 2008;Wagman, Shockley, Riley, & Turvey, 2001;Withagen & Michaels, 2005;Withagen & van Wermeskerken, 2009), visually guided braking (Fajen, 2008;Fajen & Devaney, 2006), and visually guided catching (van Hof, van der Kamp, & Savelsbergh, 2006).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%