1981
DOI: 10.1044/jshd.4604.388
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Perception of Iconicity in American Sign Language by Hearing and Deaf Subjects

Abstract: Three groups of subjects differing in age, language experience, and familiarity with American Sign Language were compared on three tasks regarding the perception of iconicity in signs from American Sign Language. Subjects were asked to guess the meaning of signs, to rate signs for iconicity, and to state connections between signs and their meaning in English. Results showed that hearing college students, deaf adults, and hearing first-grade children perform similarly on tasks regarding iconicity. Results sugge… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
12
0

Year Published

1983
1983
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 35 publications
(13 citation statements)
references
References 0 publications
1
12
0
Order By: Relevance
“…We defined iconic signs as those whose forms are motivated by the form of their referent. Given that access to signs’ iconic link greatly depends on age, world knowledge, and cultural background (Griffith et al., ; Pizzuto & Volterra, ), we operationalised iconicity through ratings from a group of 15 hearing nonsigners using the same 7‐point Likert scale as Vinson et al. () where 1 represented highly arbitrary signs and 7 highly iconic ones.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…We defined iconic signs as those whose forms are motivated by the form of their referent. Given that access to signs’ iconic link greatly depends on age, world knowledge, and cultural background (Griffith et al., ; Pizzuto & Volterra, ), we operationalised iconicity through ratings from a group of 15 hearing nonsigners using the same 7‐point Likert scale as Vinson et al. () where 1 represented highly arbitrary signs and 7 highly iconic ones.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Iconicity can be expressed in a lexical unit as a whole (Mandel, ) or it can be expressed in one or more of the signs’ phonological constituents (Cuxac, ; van der Kooij, ). Access to sign iconicity is influenced by the viewer's age, shared world knowledge (Griffith, Robinson, & Panagos, ), and cultural background (Pizzuto & Volterra, ). Thus it is not always easy to determine the referent that motivates a linguistic form.…”
Section: Acquisition Of Phonology In a Signed L2mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Signers are clearly aware of iconicity, making use of it in areas such as poetry and word play (Sutton-Spence, 2005; this is also seen in spoken languages; see the book series: Iconicity in Language and Literature, volumes 1–9, for a multitude of examples). Further, iconicity ratings have been collected for several sign languages (e.g., for BSL: Vinson et al, 2008; for ASL: Griffith et al, 1981; for ASL and DGS, Adam et al, 2007), suggesting that signers are aware of iconicity in their language and are able to make judgments about it.…”
Section: Are We Sensitive To Iconicity?mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Multiple empirical studies have consistently demonstrated that iconicity in lexical signs has a positive effect in hearing non-signers' recall, learning, naming, and translating compared to arbitrary ones (Baus, Carreiras, & Emmorey, 2012;Campbell, Martin, & White, 1992;Lieberth & Gamble, 1991). However, these findings are paradoxical given that non-signers have limited access to the iconic features of signs and perform poorly when asked to guess their meaning in isolation (Griffith, Robinson, & Panagos, 1981;Klima & Bellugi, 1979). It has been argued that hearing non-signers lack cultural and linguistic experience with a manual language and thus they cannot penetrate the iconic features of signs (Klima & Bellugi, 1979).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%