2011
DOI: 10.1177/0142723710393795
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Parental explanations of vocabulary during shared book reading: A missed opportunity

Abstract: Two studies investigated discussions of the meaning of unusual vocabulary encountered during shared book reading. In Study 1 parent-child dyads were observed longitudinally in senior kindergarten through grade 2 reading short storybooks below, at and just above the child's reading level. Here children did most of the reading. In Study 2 a second sample of parents of children in grade 1 all read the same book to their child. This book had numerous unfamiliar words, allowing an investigation of the characteristi… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
32
0

Year Published

2015
2015
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
5
4

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 39 publications
(33 citation statements)
references
References 43 publications
(58 reference statements)
1
32
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Moreover, when low‐income families offer their children exposures to uncommon vocabulary, these exposures have fewer of the high‐quality features that align with the vocabulary‐promoting cognitive and lexical process noted earlier. For example, even when families in poverty read books with their children, they are less likely to define new words for children . Similarly, give‐and‐take conversation, which allows children to practice using vocabulary and receive feedback, is less frequent .…”
Section: Understanding the Language Stimulation Gapmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Moreover, when low‐income families offer their children exposures to uncommon vocabulary, these exposures have fewer of the high‐quality features that align with the vocabulary‐promoting cognitive and lexical process noted earlier. For example, even when families in poverty read books with their children, they are less likely to define new words for children . Similarly, give‐and‐take conversation, which allows children to practice using vocabulary and receive feedback, is less frequent .…”
Section: Understanding the Language Stimulation Gapmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Children's emergent literacy skills begin to develop during the preschool years and are influenced by the nature and volume of parents' home-literacy practices (Burgess, Hecht, and Lonigan 2002;Sénéchal and LeFevre 2002;Sonnenschein and Munsterman 2002). The quality of the home-literacy environment, such as the strategies parents employ during shared book reading, can motivate children to engage during home-literacy practices which, in turn, fosters children's emergent-literacy growth (Evans et al 2011;Hammer, Miccio, and Wagstaff 2003;Roberts, Jurgens, and Burchinal 2005;Sénéchal et al 2008;Whitehurst et al 1994). Importantly, there is variation in the quality and quantity of home-literacy practices that parents employ (Phillips and Lonigan 2009;Smith and Dixon 1995), which may contribute to variation in caregivers' perceptions of their young children's motivation to participate in these practices.…”
Section: Please Scroll Down For Articlementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Part of the reason for this difficulty may be that words that are first encountered in narratives must often be learned implicitly. That is, meaning is rarely explicitly stated or highlighted within the text (Fukknik, Blok & de Glopper, ), and caregivers do not always explicitly define words encountered during shared book reading (Evans, Reynolds, Shaw & Pursoo, ). Instead, readers must intuit the meaning of words from the surrounding context (Budiu & Anderson, ), a skill with considerable individual difference (e.g., Cain & Oakhill, ).…”
Section: Highlightsmentioning
confidence: 99%