2021
DOI: 10.1007/s00426-021-01599-y
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

On preventing attention capture: Is singleton suppression actually singleton suppression?

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

7
37
2

Year Published

2022
2022
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
9

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 33 publications
(70 citation statements)
references
References 52 publications
7
37
2
Order By: Relevance
“…Rather, attention was guided away from the distractor colour via inhibition of the distractor feature value (i.e., the specific colour of the distractor; e.g., Treisman & Sato, 1990; see also Ruthruff et al, 2021), not via reactive inhibition of saliency signals (e.g., Sawaki & Luck, 2010). With this, our results are in line with the results of Lien et al (2021), who showed that suppression effects are equivalent between search arrays containing a single (thus salient) distractor and multiple distractors. Indeed, such results are consistent with findings from an older literature on visual search (e.g., Becker & Horstmann, 2009;Egeth et al, 1984;Kaptein et al, 1995;Treisman & Sato, 1990) in which participants were found to be able to limit selection to a subset of items in a conjunction search task (e.g., selecting only red items in search for a red, tilted item among red vertical and green tilted items).…”
Section: Time Course Of Distractor Benefitssupporting
confidence: 90%
“…Rather, attention was guided away from the distractor colour via inhibition of the distractor feature value (i.e., the specific colour of the distractor; e.g., Treisman & Sato, 1990; see also Ruthruff et al, 2021), not via reactive inhibition of saliency signals (e.g., Sawaki & Luck, 2010). With this, our results are in line with the results of Lien et al (2021), who showed that suppression effects are equivalent between search arrays containing a single (thus salient) distractor and multiple distractors. Indeed, such results are consistent with findings from an older literature on visual search (e.g., Becker & Horstmann, 2009;Egeth et al, 1984;Kaptein et al, 1995;Treisman & Sato, 1990) in which participants were found to be able to limit selection to a subset of items in a conjunction search task (e.g., selecting only red items in search for a red, tilted item among red vertical and green tilted items).…”
Section: Time Course Of Distractor Benefitssupporting
confidence: 90%
“…Conversely, attentional suppression is thought to prevent the allocation of attentional resources to distractor stimuli. As a result, the processing of salient distractor stimuli, as evaluated by a secondary probe task, may be worse than the processing of other nontarget stimuli (Gaspelin et al, 2015; but see Lien et al, 2021). In the context of the present study, attentional suppression of the high-frequency distractor position may have prevented the erroneous allocation of attentional resources to distractors appearing at this location.…”
Section: Underlying Mechanismsmentioning
confidence: 67%
“…Because search performance in this variant of the additional singleton paradigm was better in the presence than in the absence of the salient distractor, Gaspelin et al, concluded that the suppression of the salient distractor below baseline accounted for the improved search performance. However, Lien et al (2021) observed that below-baseline performance in the search task occurred even for nonsalient nontargets, which contradicts the idea that the effect arises from suppression of salient distractor stimuli. Further, it was proposed that an electrophysiological component associated with attentional suppression, the P D component (Hickey et al, 2009), occurred in response to a salient distractor that was successfully suppressed (Gaspelin & Luck, 2018).…”
Section: Underlying Mechanismmentioning
confidence: 74%