1991
DOI: 10.1037/0033-2909.109.2.242
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Odor memory: Taking stock.

Abstract: This review discusses the state of knowledge in odor memory within the framework of mainstream memory research. Experimental findings are structured around prevailing theoretical distinctions in the study of memory proper, including semantic-episodic memory and implicit-explicit memory. Unaided odor-identification performance is found to be approximately 40%-50% of the total stimulus set presented to Ss, although performance approaches the limit of memorial discrimination if Ss are given label training with fe… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

5
85
0
5

Year Published

2002
2002
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
4
2

Relationship

0
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 156 publications
(95 citation statements)
references
References 78 publications
(106 reference statements)
5
85
0
5
Order By: Relevance
“…Although our primary interest in the present study was in odors that could not be identified at test, it is important to first consider how often the participants were able to identify the odors at test. As can be seen in Table 1, identification of the scents at test was fairly difficult, which is consistent with reports in the literature that naming scents is more difficult than naming other types of stimuli (e.g., Cain, 1979;Herz & Engen, 1996;Schab, 1991;Stevenson et al, 2007). Despite this overall difficulty in naming scents, a 2 (study status: studied vs. nonstudied) 2 (study condition: name only vs. name plus scent) mixed model ANOVA performed on the proportion of scents identified revealed a main effect of study status such that more test scents were identified overall when either their names or their names and the scents themselves were studied [F(1,66) Just as identifying word fragments or rapidly flashed and masked stimuli might be considered implicit memory tasks (e.g., Roediger & McDermott, 1993), so might identifying scents.…”
Section: Resultssupporting
confidence: 79%
See 4 more Smart Citations
“…Although our primary interest in the present study was in odors that could not be identified at test, it is important to first consider how often the participants were able to identify the odors at test. As can be seen in Table 1, identification of the scents at test was fairly difficult, which is consistent with reports in the literature that naming scents is more difficult than naming other types of stimuli (e.g., Cain, 1979;Herz & Engen, 1996;Schab, 1991;Stevenson et al, 2007). Despite this overall difficulty in naming scents, a 2 (study status: studied vs. nonstudied) 2 (study condition: name only vs. name plus scent) mixed model ANOVA performed on the proportion of scents identified revealed a main effect of study status such that more test scents were identified overall when either their names or their names and the scents themselves were studied [F(1,66) Just as identifying word fragments or rapidly flashed and masked stimuli might be considered implicit memory tasks (e.g., Roediger & McDermott, 1993), so might identifying scents.…”
Section: Resultssupporting
confidence: 79%
“…Although some prior research suggests that odor imagery involves representations of the actual odors themselves (e.g., Djordjevic, Zatorre, Petrides, & Jones-Gotman, 2004), Stevenson et al demonstrated that names more easily induced imagery of their visual referents (e.g., an image of a strawberry) than of their odor referents (e.g., the imagined smell of a strawberry). Furthermore, in line with prior research (see Herz & Engen, 1996, or Schab, 1991, for a review), Stevenson et al demonstrated that whereas visual images (e.g., an image of a strawberry) are easily named, odors (e.g., the smell of strawberry) are not as easily named.…”
supporting
confidence: 49%
See 3 more Smart Citations