2010
DOI: 10.3758/mc.38.4.452
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Odor recognition without identification

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

0
28
0

Year Published

2013
2013
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 37 publications
(28 citation statements)
references
References 37 publications
(110 reference statements)
0
28
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In short, during TOT states, people are biased to judge unretrieved targets as studied. This TOT bias is distinguishable from recognition-without-identification, occurring in situations where the recognition-without-identification effect does not (e.g., Cleary et al, 2010), and even in situations where the recognition-without-identification effect is reversed (Cleary et al, 2014).…”
Section: Members Of Underrepresented Groups: Reviewers For Journal Mamentioning
confidence: 94%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…In short, during TOT states, people are biased to judge unretrieved targets as studied. This TOT bias is distinguishable from recognition-without-identification, occurring in situations where the recognition-without-identification effect does not (e.g., Cleary et al, 2010), and even in situations where the recognition-without-identification effect is reversed (Cleary et al, 2014).…”
Section: Members Of Underrepresented Groups: Reviewers For Journal Mamentioning
confidence: 94%
“…The font color was set using the E-prime software's black and silver font color settings, respectively. Although there were only three conditions (studied in dark font, studied in light font, and nonstudied), we kept the ratio of studied to nonstudied items equal, as in prior research (e.g., Cleary & Specker, 2007;Cleary, 2006;Cleary et al, 2010;Cleary & Reyes, 2009). Therefore, to simplify our counterbalancing, four versions of the experiment were created to rotate the answer through the conditions of studied versus nonstudied and dark versus light font across participants.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Some feature of the test cue was connected to the episodic memory representation, giving rise to a sense of familiarity for that cue. The finding has been subsequently replicated across a wide variety of cue materials, including odors (Cleary, Konkel, Nomi, & McCabe, 2010), music (Kostic & Cleary, 2009), famous landmarks (Cleary & Reyes, 2009), simple line drawings (Cleary, Langley, & Seiler, 2004), and celebrity faces (Cleary & Specker, 2007). For example, participants are able to identify that a famous face shown at test had the associated name presented previously during input (above chance), even though they cannot retrieve the name (Cleary & Specker 2007).…”
mentioning
confidence: 72%
“…The automatic priming of such information may be a key component underlying confidence ratings and TOT assessments. In fact, Cleary has likened the RWI phenomenon to the TOT experience (Cleary, 2006;Cleary et al, 2010;Cleary & Reyes, 2009;Cleary & Specker, 2007), assuming that sensing that a word is stored in semantic memory may be similar to feeling that a word appeared in the prior list.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%