2011
DOI: 10.3758/s13423-011-0078-9
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Observing neighborhood effects without neighbors

Abstract: With a new metric called phonological Levenshtein distance (PLD20), the present study explores the effects of phonological similarity and word frequency on spoken word recognition, using polysyllabic words that have neither phonological nor orthographic neighbors, as defined by neighborhood density (the N-metric). Inhibitory effects of PLD20 were observed for these lexical hermits: Close-PLD20 words were recognized more slowly than distant PLD20 words, indicating lexical competition. Importantly, these inhibit… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
45
1

Year Published

2013
2013
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
8
1
1

Relationship

1
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 38 publications
(47 citation statements)
references
References 19 publications
1
45
1
Order By: Relevance
“…However, we showed that words that are phonologically similar to many other words (i.e., with low PLD20 values) were responded to faster than phonologically dissimilar words (i.e., with high PLD20 values). This latter result departs from the general finding that, in the auditory modality, words with more similar sounding (or closer phonological neighbors) are usually recognized more slowly than more distinct word-forms (e.g., Goh et al, 2009;Goh et al, 2016;Suárez, Tan, Yap, & Goh, 2011;Ziegler et al, 2003), but it converges with the general finding that, in the visual modality, words that are orthographically similar to many other words are responded to faster than orthographically dissimilar words (e.g., Brysbaert et al, 2016;Ferrand et al, 2010;Keuleers, Diependaele, & Brysbaert, 2010;Keuleers et al, 2012). Concerning phonological uniqueness point, our results are consistent with the general finding that words with an early phonological uniqueness point are responded to faster than words with a late phonological uniqueness point (e.g., Radeau & Morais, 1990;Radeau et al, 2000;Radeau et al, 1989).…”
Section: Summary Of Findingscontrasting
confidence: 84%
“…However, we showed that words that are phonologically similar to many other words (i.e., with low PLD20 values) were responded to faster than phonologically dissimilar words (i.e., with high PLD20 values). This latter result departs from the general finding that, in the auditory modality, words with more similar sounding (or closer phonological neighbors) are usually recognized more slowly than more distinct word-forms (e.g., Goh et al, 2009;Goh et al, 2016;Suárez, Tan, Yap, & Goh, 2011;Ziegler et al, 2003), but it converges with the general finding that, in the visual modality, words that are orthographically similar to many other words are responded to faster than orthographically dissimilar words (e.g., Brysbaert et al, 2016;Ferrand et al, 2010;Keuleers, Diependaele, & Brysbaert, 2010;Keuleers et al, 2012). Concerning phonological uniqueness point, our results are consistent with the general finding that words with an early phonological uniqueness point are responded to faster than words with a late phonological uniqueness point (e.g., Radeau & Morais, 1990;Radeau et al, 2000;Radeau et al, 1989).…”
Section: Summary Of Findingscontrasting
confidence: 84%
“…Therefore, it is sensible to assume that our bilinguals must have used their experience with English and suprasegmental correlates of lexical stress, such as pitch and intensity, while processing stressed and unstressed syllables within words. Finally, previous research on the effects of word frequency and lexical competition during spoken word recognition has used Singaporean samples, and the results have yielded results comparable to those obtained with English monolingual samples (e.g., Suárez, Tan, Yap, & Goh, 2011).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 58%
“…More interesting, over 95% of the responses that differed from the target word by more than a single phoneme had a path that connected the target word to the more distant response, such as being presented with dog and responding with bag (see Figure 1). The existence of lexical intermediaries between the target word and more distant responses raises some concerns about measures of word-form similarity that ignore such items, such as the Orthographic Levenshtein Distance-20 ( OLD-20 ; Yarkoni, Balota & Yap, 2008), and the Phonological Levenshtein Distance-20 ( PLD-20 ; Suárez, Tan, Yap & Goh, 2011). Computations of OLD-20 / PLD-20 do not consider whether real-word intermediaries exist or not; the measure only considers the number of letter/phoneme changes (respectively) that are required to turn one word into another.…”
Section: How Lexical Structure Influences Lexical Processingmentioning
confidence: 99%