2013
DOI: 10.3758/s13428-013-0319-0
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Objective age of acquisition norms for a set of 286 words in Russian: Relationships with other psycholinguistic variables

Abstract: Normative data on the objective age of acquisition (AoA) for 286 Russian words are presented in this article. In addition, correlations between the objective AoA and subjective ratings, name agreement, picture name agreement, imageability, familiarity, word frequency, and word length are provided, as are correlations between the objective AoA and two measures of exemplar dominance (exemplar generation frequency and the number of times an exemplar was named first). The correlations between the aforementioned va… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
4
1

Citation Types

0
11
0

Year Published

2017
2017
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 18 publications
(13 citation statements)
references
References 47 publications
0
11
0
Order By: Relevance
“…There are two main approaches to derive AoA data. First, objective AoA measures can be obtained by analysis of children’s production (Chalard et al, 2003; Álvarez and Cuetos, 2007; Lotto et al, 2010; Grigoriev and Oshhepkov, 2013). Within this approach, children (classified by age) are asked to name the picture of common objects and activities.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…There are two main approaches to derive AoA data. First, objective AoA measures can be obtained by analysis of children’s production (Chalard et al, 2003; Álvarez and Cuetos, 2007; Lotto et al, 2010; Grigoriev and Oshhepkov, 2013). Within this approach, children (classified by age) are asked to name the picture of common objects and activities.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…ru/ru_stimul/) and a database of pictures of 375 action nouns and verbs (Akinina et al 2015, http://en.stimdb.ru/). The norms for age of acquisition, name agreement, picture name agreement, imageability, familiarity, word frequency, and word length for the most widely used picture set in general psycholinguistics by Snodgrass and Vanderwart (1980) are also available for Russian (Bonin, Guillemard-Tsarapina, and Méot 2013;Grigoriev and Oshhepkov 2013) and Croatian (Rogić et al 2013). Lists of pseudowords are very useful for crossmodal priming experiments that rely on a lexical decision task; one such list is available for Polish (Imbir, Spustek, and Żygierewicz 2015).…”
Section: Resources For Slavic Psycholinguisticsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Normative descriptions of words or expressions are available in many languages, including Chinese (Chang et al, 2016), Dutch (Hubers et al, 2019), English (Juhasz et al, 2015), French (Bonin et al, 2015), German (Schulte Im Walde & Borgwaldt, 2015), Hebrew (Yael et al, 2015), Italian (Barca et al, 2002), Malay (Yap et al, 2010), Russian (Grigoriev & Oshhepkov, 2013), Spanish (Guasch et al, 2016), and Swedish (Blomberg & Öberg, 2015). Probably due to the complexity of vocabulary per se, this kind of study is typically conducted on samples of words or expressions in a language.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%