2015
DOI: 10.3758/s13414-015-0995-1
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Novelty biases attention and gaze in a surprise trial

Abstract: While the classical distinction between taskdriven and stimulus-driven biasing of attention appears to be a dichotomy at first sight, there seems to be a third category that depends on the contrast or discrepancy between active representations and the upcoming stimulus, and may be termed novelty, surprise, or prediction failure. For previous demonstrations of the discrepancy-attention link, stimulus-driven components (saliency) may have played a decisive role. The present study was conducted to evaluate the di… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
31
0
2

Year Published

2016
2016
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6
2

Relationship

2
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 61 publications
(35 citation statements)
references
References 35 publications
(47 reference statements)
2
31
0
2
Order By: Relevance
“…The present results are in line with previous work (Becker & Horstmann, 2011, Experiment 3) that attention is biased towards the salient stimulus even if it is not singled out by feature novelty, and clarify the time course of this effect. Interestingly, the time course of the deployment of gaze is similar to previous studies from the surprise paradigm, when the singleton was the only element in the critical trial with a novel feature (Horstmann & Herwig, 2015), and when nonsalient novel stimuli are presented in the critical trial (Horstmann & Herwig, 2016). The similarity in time course is suggestive of a common origin.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 79%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The present results are in line with previous work (Becker & Horstmann, 2011, Experiment 3) that attention is biased towards the salient stimulus even if it is not singled out by feature novelty, and clarify the time course of this effect. Interestingly, the time course of the deployment of gaze is similar to previous studies from the surprise paradigm, when the singleton was the only element in the critical trial with a novel feature (Horstmann & Herwig, 2015), and when nonsalient novel stimuli are presented in the critical trial (Horstmann & Herwig, 2016). The similarity in time course is suggestive of a common origin.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 79%
“…Correspondingly, showed that apparent motion captured attention following pre-critical trials without apparent motion, but not after pre-critical trials where in some displays all items showed apparent motion. In addition to these demonstrations that salience is not sufficient, salience does also not seem to be necessary to attract attention: Horstmann and Herwig (2016) demonstrated that the eyes are attracted by a novel feature even when the feature was not a singleton in the display.…”
Section: Evidence From Unexpected Singletonsmentioning
confidence: 97%
“…Specific behavioral evidence for the attentional shift caused by surprising events was obtained in a series of eye‐tracking studies which found that surprising stimuli attract gaze in a visual search task (Horstmann & Herwig, , ; see Horstmann, , for a review of the surprise‐attention link). The average latency of the first gaze contact with the surprising stimulus occurred around 400 ms, which is consistent with the time course of detection deficits caused by surprising events in a rapid serial visual presentation task (e.g.…”
Section: The Effects Of Surprisementioning
confidence: 99%
“…1; see Horstmann, ). When multiple objects with a schema‐discrepant feature are presented, the preference for this feature may extend over multiple fixations (Horstmann & Herwig, ).…”
Section: The Effects Of Surprisementioning
confidence: 99%
“…As a first objective, we aimed to determine whether the high level of endogenous alert, that we assume to occur in the optimal time of day according to the individual’s chronotype, improves performance in both a rather monotonous task that is supposed to activate the vigilant attentional network (the PVT), and in a conflict demanding task that requires sustained attention throughout the task (the flanker task). Additionally, as attention is thought to be biased to novel stimulus and/or locations 27 , 28 , we assessed whether synchrony effects in the two aforementioned tasks can be modulated by task-related novelty, that is, first-experience-with-task occurring in the first session. It might be that first-experience-with-task increases alerting levels, compensating the low endogenous alertness when participants complete the tasks for the first time at their non-optimal time.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%