2019
DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2018.09.001
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Non-linguistic effects of language switching training

Abstract: What is the relationship between bilingual language control (BLC) mechanisms and domain-general executive control (EC) processes? Do these two domains share some of their mechanisms? Here, we take a novel approach to this question, investigating whether short-term language switching training improves non-linguistic task switching performance. Two groups of bilinguals were assigned to two different protocols; one group was trained in language switching (switching-task training group) another group was trained i… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

7
47
2

Year Published

2019
2019
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 54 publications
(59 citation statements)
references
References 61 publications
7
47
2
Order By: Relevance
“…Moreover, they observed significant correlations between language-and task-mixing costs with short and long cue-to-stimulus intervals (r ranging from .379 to .421). Along the same lines, Prior and Gollan (2013) observed a significant correlation between language-and task-mixing costs (r = .45), as did Timmer, Calabria, and Costa (2019) after the bilingual participants had language switching training (r = .273). Timmer et al (2019), however, found no significant correlation between the two types of mixing costs in the pretraining phase (r = .198).…”
Section: Relationship Between Proactive Language Control and Proactivmentioning
confidence: 65%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Moreover, they observed significant correlations between language-and task-mixing costs with short and long cue-to-stimulus intervals (r ranging from .379 to .421). Along the same lines, Prior and Gollan (2013) observed a significant correlation between language-and task-mixing costs (r = .45), as did Timmer, Calabria, and Costa (2019) after the bilingual participants had language switching training (r = .273). Timmer et al (2019), however, found no significant correlation between the two types of mixing costs in the pretraining phase (r = .198).…”
Section: Relationship Between Proactive Language Control and Proactivmentioning
confidence: 65%
“…Along the same lines, Prior and Gollan (2013) observed a significant correlation between language-and task-mixing costs (r = .45), as did Timmer, Calabria, and Costa (2019) after the bilingual participants had language switching training (r = .273). Timmer et al (2019), however, found no significant correlation between the two types of mixing costs in the pretraining phase (r = .198). Stasenko et al (2017) found additional evidence for a difference, as language-mixing costs increased from the first to the second half of the experiment, whereas task-mixing costs decreased from the first to the second half of the experiment.…”
Section: Relationship Between Proactive Language Control and Proactivmentioning
confidence: 65%
“…in older adults Bialystok et al, 2004Bialystok et al, , 2008; for reviews see Lehtonen et al, 2018;Paap et al, 2015;Valian, 2015). The standing hypothesis is that bilinguals continuously use language control mechanisms to avoid cross-language interference and this lifelong training in control would transfer its benefits to domain-general cognitive processes, resulting in increased efficiency within the EC system (Prior & Gollan, 2013;Timmer, Calabria, & Costa, 2019). Therefore, one could expect that lifelong training of actively speaking two languages would be beneficial in counteracting the negative effects of cognitive decline in bilinguals.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Lastly, Timmer, Calabria, and Costa (2019) found that training on a bilingual switching task led to improvement in a general switching task. This could be taken to support the taskgenerality hypothesis, but it is also in line with the skill learning theories (and Paap's controlled dose hypothesis in particular), which imply that general EF are employed only during initial stages of language learning.…”
Section: Cued Language Switching Performance In a Laboratory Task Andmentioning
confidence: 96%