2009
DOI: 10.1167/9.11.3
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

No capacity limit in attentional tracking: Evidence for probabilistic inference under a resource constraint

Abstract: Human ability to simultaneously track multiple items declines with set size. This effect is commonly attributed to a fixed limit on the number of items that can be attended to, a notion that is formalized in limited-capacity and slot models. Instead, we propose that observers are constrained by stimulus uncertainty that increases with the number of items but use Bayesian inference to achieve optimal performance. We model five data sets from published deviation discrimination experiments that varied set size, n… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

6
60
0

Year Published

2010
2010
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 50 publications
(66 citation statements)
references
References 47 publications
(100 reference statements)
6
60
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The most significant performancelimiting factor in any form of MOT may be the uncertainty associated with represented object locations, relative to the proximity of other objects that might steal the spotlight of activation and foil ongoing correspondence computations (Franconeri, Jonathan, & Scimeca, 2010;Franconeri, Scimeca, & Jonathan, 2012;Franconeri et al, 2008;Ma & Huang, 2009;Vul, Frank, Alvarez, & Tenenbaum, 2009). Our results cannot rule out a role for motion extrapolation across other types of disruptions or for tracking performance without disruptions.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The most significant performancelimiting factor in any form of MOT may be the uncertainty associated with represented object locations, relative to the proximity of other objects that might steal the spotlight of activation and foil ongoing correspondence computations (Franconeri, Jonathan, & Scimeca, 2010;Franconeri, Scimeca, & Jonathan, 2012;Franconeri et al, 2008;Ma & Huang, 2009;Vul, Frank, Alvarez, & Tenenbaum, 2009). Our results cannot rule out a role for motion extrapolation across other types of disruptions or for tracking performance without disruptions.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The inclusion of nontarget measurements is critical, as the typical MOT task would be trivial if the observer knew which portions of the moment-by-moment stimulus were due to targets. All of the observations are corrupted by independent noise; as is standard in Bayesian approaches to perception, we assume that the observer is aware of her own noise variance (e.g., Girshick, Landy, & Simoncelli, 2011;Kersten, Mamassian, & Yuille, 2004;Lee & Mumford, 2003;Ma & Huang, 2009;Maloney, 2002;van den Berg, Shin, Chou, George, & Ma, 2012;Vul, Frank, Alvarez, & Tenenbaum, 2009; in the case of motion, specifically, see Sekuler, Watamaniuk, & Blake, 2002;Warren, Graf, Champion, & Maloney, 2012). 2.…”
Section: General Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The Kalman filter is a basic framework for recursively estimating the values of unknown variables from noisy measurements. It is applied frequently in computer vision applications for tracking (BarShalom, & Fortmann, 1988;Boykov & Huttenlocher, 2000), and it supplies the main framework for two prior modeling efforts in MOT (Ma & Huang, 2009;Vul, Frank, Alvarez, & Tenenbaum, 2009). Using the Kalman filter as a computational foundation, we begin by implementing three models of multiple object tracking, one that does not utilize extrapolated predictions, and two models that weight extrapolations in different ways.…”
Section: The Current Studymentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations