2006
DOI: 10.1207/s15327078in0903_5
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Nineteen‐Month‐Olds' Understanding of the Conventionality of Object Labels Versus Desires

Abstract: We assessed 19‐month‐olds' appreciation of the conventional nature of object labels versus desires. Infants played a finding game with an experimenter who stated her intention to find the referent of a novel word (word group), to find an object she wanted (desire group), or simply to look in a box (control group). A 2nd experimenter then administered a comprehension task to assess infants' tendency to extend information to a 2nd person who was not present at the time of learning. Results indicate that infants … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

5
71
2

Year Published

2007
2007
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
9
1

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 80 publications
(78 citation statements)
references
References 19 publications
5
71
2
Order By: Relevance
“…This finding is also consistent with results suggesting that by the second half of the first year, infants generalize object labels across contexts (e.g., Henderson & Graham, 2005;Graham, Stock, & Henderson, 2006;Henderson & Woodward, 2012;Novack, Henderson, & Woodward, 2014). More generally, these data are consistent with the hypothesis that young children are sensitive to social cues that signal communicative intent, and that such cues might guide learning behavior (e.g., Csibra, 2010;Csibra & Gergeley, 2009 Baldwin, 1993;Csibra & Gergeley, 2009).…”
Section: This Finding Corroborates and Extends Work By Brooker Andsupporting
confidence: 86%
“…This finding is also consistent with results suggesting that by the second half of the first year, infants generalize object labels across contexts (e.g., Henderson & Graham, 2005;Graham, Stock, & Henderson, 2006;Henderson & Woodward, 2012;Novack, Henderson, & Woodward, 2014). More generally, these data are consistent with the hypothesis that young children are sensitive to social cues that signal communicative intent, and that such cues might guide learning behavior (e.g., Csibra, 2010;Csibra & Gergeley, 2009 Baldwin, 1993;Csibra & Gergeley, 2009).…”
Section: This Finding Corroborates and Extends Work By Brooker Andsupporting
confidence: 86%
“…Research has shown that from a very young age, infants recognize that language is conventional (e.g., Buresh & Woodward, 2007a;Diesendruck & Markson, 2001;Graham, Stock, & Henderson, 2006; see also Koenig & Echols, 2003) but that goals are not (Buresh & Woodward, 2007a), nor are desires (Graham et al, 2006), nor are personal facts about an object (e.g., ''My uncle gave this to me'') (Diesendruck & Markson, 2001). Children as young as 2 and 3 years have also demonstrated an understanding that conventions play a role in games, and as such the rules of a game should not be broken (Rakoczy, Warneken, & Tomasello, 2008; see also Rakoczy, Brosche, Warneken, & Tomasello, 2009).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…There is evidence that young children have some understanding of this dependency. Infants expect two people to share knowledge of common nouns (Buresh & Woodward, 2007;Graham, Stock, & Henderson, 2006;Henderson & Graham, 2005;Henderson & Woodward, 2012) but this expectation of shared knowledge does not extend to proper names (Diesendruck, 2005) or preferences (Fawcett & Markson, 2010a). At the level of specific languages, infants distinguish between the sounds (Mehler, Jusczyk, Lambertz, Halsted, Bertoncini, & Amiel-Tison, 1988) and speakers of their own and other languages (Kinzler, Dupoux, & Spelke, 2007), and prefer speakers of their own language (e.g., Kinzler et al, 2007;Kinzler, Dupoux, & Spelke, 2012).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%