1994
DOI: 10.1016/s0079-6123(08)60550-6
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Neural networks in the brain involved in memory and recall

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

0
32
0

Year Published

1998
1998
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
10

Relationship

1
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 64 publications
(32 citation statements)
references
References 14 publications
0
32
0
Order By: Relevance
“…One possible scenario is that hippocampal activity patterns during a given 50% morph trial reflect reinstatements of the entire original learnt representation (e.g., 100% scene A), a process consistent with the operation of a bistable attractor network, and one that might plausibly be viewed to induce participants to choose one response over another. Indeed neural network models have often favored the idea that memories are stored as discrete local attractors (Hopfield,1982; Rolls and Treves,1994), with partial or ambiguous inputs (50% morphs) abruptly inducing the network to occupy one state (e.g., relating to 100% A) or the other (e.g., relating to 100% B) through “global” pattern completion (i.e., output of entire stored pattern). However, the failure of a classifier trained on 50% morphs to correctly assign labels to 100% scenes (see previous analysis), argues strongly against this scenario.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…One possible scenario is that hippocampal activity patterns during a given 50% morph trial reflect reinstatements of the entire original learnt representation (e.g., 100% scene A), a process consistent with the operation of a bistable attractor network, and one that might plausibly be viewed to induce participants to choose one response over another. Indeed neural network models have often favored the idea that memories are stored as discrete local attractors (Hopfield,1982; Rolls and Treves,1994), with partial or ambiguous inputs (50% morphs) abruptly inducing the network to occupy one state (e.g., relating to 100% A) or the other (e.g., relating to 100% B) through “global” pattern completion (i.e., output of entire stored pattern). However, the failure of a classifier trained on 50% morphs to correctly assign labels to 100% scenes (see previous analysis), argues strongly against this scenario.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In contrast, the reactivation-related expression of zif268 in the DG during rest is relatively preserved, and we hypothesize that this activity is mediated by the backprojection from CA3. This hypothesis is supported by the fact that both computational models (e.g., [32-34]) and experimental evidence (e.g., [34-38]) support CA3 as the most likely source of recapitulation of recent activity patterns. This is thought to stimulate DG either through direct backprojections from CA3c (nearest the hilus), or through indirect projections via mossy cells [39,40].…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 97%
“…It will be important in future studies to further define the anatomical loci from which the observed pattern completion deficits originate. It would be especially interesting to investigate candidate sites such as the anterior cingular cortex, the temporal cortex and the hippocampus using pharmacological, genetic, and large-scale in vivo recording techniques [11], [41][44]. It is also important to assess whether genetic compensation or slow changes in the mutant brain contribute to the observed recall deficits.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%