2013
DOI: 10.1017/s0140525x12002476
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Navigating in a three-dimensional world

Abstract: The study of spatial cognition has provided considerable insight into how animals (including humans) navigate on the horizontal plane. However, the real world is three-dimensional, having a complex topography including both horizontal and vertical features, which presents additional challenges for representation and navigation. The present article reviews the emerging behavioral and neurobiological literature on spatial cognition in non-horizontal environments. We suggest that three-dimensional spaces are repr… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

7
88
1

Year Published

2014
2014
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
7
2

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 112 publications
(99 citation statements)
references
References 302 publications
(443 reference statements)
7
88
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Together with the behavioral findings of high accuracy regardless of the presence of a vertical or horizontal distractor and the symmetrically perceived length of the 3D environment, multivoxel representational similarity in the right aHC supports an isotropic 3D space encoding hypothesis and is evidence against the anisotropic planar encoding hypothesis (Hayman et al, 2011; Jeffery et al, 2013). …”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 60%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Together with the behavioral findings of high accuracy regardless of the presence of a vertical or horizontal distractor and the symmetrically perceived length of the 3D environment, multivoxel representational similarity in the right aHC supports an isotropic 3D space encoding hypothesis and is evidence against the anisotropic planar encoding hypothesis (Hayman et al, 2011; Jeffery et al, 2013). …”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 60%
“…Hayman et al (2011) recorded place and grid cells in the HC and EC of rats moving on a vertical wall and helix staircase. In both apparatuses, place and grid cells expressed less information about the vertical axis, leading the investigators to propose that 3D space representation might be fundamentally planar (Jeffery et al, 2013). In contrast, most place cells recorded in CA1 of flying bats were equally sensitive to all three axes, suggesting isotropic representation of 3D space (Yartsev and Ulanovsky, 2013; for review, see Finkelstein et al, 2016; for a review of fish behavior in 3D, see Burt de Perera et al, 2016).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In a three-dimensional world with rich topography, vertically extended surfaces should not be ignored (for a review on navigation in 3D, see Jeffery et al 2013). Terrain slope provides surface-traveling animals with a directional spatial cue that aids navigation in large-scale environments.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This finding will undoubtedly prompt a careful re-examination of head-direction cells in rodents, and fresh investigations in other species. Other mammals may use different compasses 10 , and if they do, understanding what drives the differences between species (whether they are hard-wired at birth or develop with experience) will lead to insight into the basic principles of the system. ■ …”
Section: A Three-dimensional Neural Compassmentioning
confidence: 99%