2022
DOI: 10.1101/2022.01.12.475866
|View full text |Cite
Preprint
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Multimodal cues displayed by submissive rats promote prosocial choices by dominants

Abstract: Animals often display prosocial behaviours, performing actions that benefit others. Although prosociality is essential for social bonding and cooperation, we still know very little about how animals integrate behavioural cues from those in need to make decisions that increase their wellbeing. To address this question, we used a two-choice task where rats can provide rewards to a conspecific in the absence of self-benefit, and interrogated which conditions promote prosociality by manipulating the social context… Show more

Help me understand this report
View published versions

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
4
0

Year Published

2022
2022
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
2
1

Relationship

0
3

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 3 publications
(4 citation statements)
references
References 76 publications
0
4
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Yet, interestingly, the observed food provisioning rate ($69%) of the Japanese macaques in our study resembled cooperative breeding and self-domesticated species' provisioning, 8,10,15 notably while using the exact same set-up and procedure. These results, together with the findings of prosocial preferences (although using different set-up and/or procedures) among other non-domesticated species and species that lack cooperative breeding or allomaternal care, 24,26,27,[66][67][68] call for additional hypotheses explaining the evolution of prosocial preferences.…”
Section: Genuine Prosocial Intentmentioning
confidence: 79%
“…Yet, interestingly, the observed food provisioning rate ($69%) of the Japanese macaques in our study resembled cooperative breeding and self-domesticated species' provisioning, 8,10,15 notably while using the exact same set-up and procedure. These results, together with the findings of prosocial preferences (although using different set-up and/or procedures) among other non-domesticated species and species that lack cooperative breeding or allomaternal care, 24,26,27,[66][67][68] call for additional hypotheses explaining the evolution of prosocial preferences.…”
Section: Genuine Prosocial Intentmentioning
confidence: 79%
“…Yet, interestingly, the observed food provisioning rate (∼ 69%) of the Japanese macaques in our study resembled cooperative breeding and self-domesticated species’ prosocial provisioning (3, 5, 10), notably while using the exact same set-up and procedure. These results, together with the findings of prosociality (although used different set-up and/or procedures) among other non-domesticated species and species that lack cooperative breeding or allomaternal care (16, 18, 19, 5456), call for additional hypotheses explaining the evolution of prosocial preferences.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 82%
“…These results, together with the findings of prosociality (although used different set-up and/or procedures) among other non-domesticated species and species that lack cooperative breeding or allomaternal care (16,18,19,(54)(55)(56), call for additional hypotheses explaining the evolution of prosocial preferences.…”
Section: The Evolution Of Prosociality the Influence Of Socio-ecologi...mentioning
confidence: 83%
“…Still, there is great variability in prosociality across rats, with about one-third failing to develop any preference and the remaining displaying a prosocial preference varying from 60% to 90%. Such variance may in part be explained by social dominance since dominant rats seem more prosocial than their submissive cagemates [144], but not by familiarity [144]. Choosing between a lever that delivers food to the focal animal or to both animals also generates a modest preference for the prosocial option in rats [37].…”
Section: Other-regarding Preferencesmentioning
confidence: 99%