2012
DOI: 10.1177/0267658312455822
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Multicompetence and native speaker variation in clausal packaging in Japanese

Abstract: Native speakers show systematic variation in a range of linguistic domains as a function of a variety of sociolinguistic variables. This paper addresses native language variation in the context of multicompetence, i.e. knowledge of two languages in one mind (Cook, 1991). Descriptions of motion were elicited from functionally monolingual and nonmonolingual speakers of Japanese, with analyses focusing on clausal packaging of Manner and Path. Results revealed that (1) acquisition of a second language (L2) appears… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
19
1
1

Year Published

2014
2014
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
7
3

Relationship

1
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 25 publications
(23 citation statements)
references
References 87 publications
(173 reference statements)
2
19
1
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Some research has failed to find evidence of bidirectional cross‐linguistic interactions, even in aspects of motion event expression (e.g., Cadierno, ; Pavlenko, ; Pavlenko & Volynsky, ). In addition, conflicting findings exist on the impact of L2 proficiency and immersion experience (e.g., Athanasopoulos, , ; Bylund & Athanasopoulos, ; Cook et al, ; versus Brown & Gullberg, , ; Chen, ; Su, ), particularly with respect to how interactions may change over the course of language development (see Stam, versus 2015; and discussion in Brown & Gullberg, ). Finally, competing models have been proposed to account for the phenomenon of convergence (see discussions in e.g., Alferink & Gullberg, ; Brown & Gullberg, ).…”
Section: General Discussion and Conclusionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Some research has failed to find evidence of bidirectional cross‐linguistic interactions, even in aspects of motion event expression (e.g., Cadierno, ; Pavlenko, ; Pavlenko & Volynsky, ). In addition, conflicting findings exist on the impact of L2 proficiency and immersion experience (e.g., Athanasopoulos, , ; Bylund & Athanasopoulos, ; Cook et al, ; versus Brown & Gullberg, , ; Chen, ; Su, ), particularly with respect to how interactions may change over the course of language development (see Stam, versus 2015; and discussion in Brown & Gullberg, ). Finally, competing models have been proposed to account for the phenomenon of convergence (see discussions in e.g., Alferink & Gullberg, ; Brown & Gullberg, ).…”
Section: General Discussion and Conclusionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…These participants can be characterized as functional monolinguals because they were not studying English or any other L2 at the time of data collection and because the only languages they used in their daily lives were Spanish and Danish, respectively (cf. Brown & Gullberg, ). Neither group reported knowledge of the other language.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Instead, when more sensitive measures of proficiency are used (such as the OQPT or tests of specific proficiency), proficiency effects are more likely to become apparent. We adhere to Brown and Gullberg's (2012) observation that the use of standardized proficiency tests across different studies has a great potential in clarifying the effects of this variable and would also allow for postcomparisons between different study populations. In addition to establishing general proficiency levels, we believe that careful assessments of participants' mastery of the specific linguistic property that gives rise to cognitive differences constitute a promising way forward, given that those studies using this approach have all yielded significant results.…”
Section: Language Proficiencymentioning
confidence: 99%