2013
DOI: 10.3389/fncom.2013.00017
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Multi-stable perception balances stability and sensitivity

Abstract: We report that multi-stable perception operates in a consistent, dynamical regime, balancing the conflicting goals of stability and sensitivity. When a multi-stable visual display is viewed continuously, its phenomenal appearance reverses spontaneously at irregular intervals. We characterized the perceptual dynamics of individual observers in terms of four statistical measures: the distribution of dominance times (mean and variance) and the novel, subtle dependence on prior history (correlation and time-consta… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
66
0

Year Published

2014
2014
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
4
3

Relationship

1
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 47 publications
(66 citation statements)
references
References 87 publications
0
66
0
Order By: Relevance
“…It has been shown to be rather similar for humans and non-human primates and together with the degree of serial correlation between dominance times it has been an important element in the debate on the relative roles of neural adaptation and neural noise in driving perceptual alternations (Fox & Herrmann, 1967;Moreno-Bote et al, 2007;Pastukhov et al, 2013;Shpiro et al, 2009;van Ee, 2009). On a general level, the distribution's peaked, rather than monotonically decreasing, shape indicates that the probability of a perceptual switch is low during the time period immediately following the previous switch, a temporary stability that may reflect a lack of accumulated adaptation or some other stabilizing mechanism (Kloosterman et al, 2014;Moreno-Bote et al, 2007;Pastukhov et al, 2013).…”
Section: A Note On the Distribution Of Dominance Timesmentioning
confidence: 97%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…It has been shown to be rather similar for humans and non-human primates and together with the degree of serial correlation between dominance times it has been an important element in the debate on the relative roles of neural adaptation and neural noise in driving perceptual alternations (Fox & Herrmann, 1967;Moreno-Bote et al, 2007;Pastukhov et al, 2013;Shpiro et al, 2009;van Ee, 2009). On a general level, the distribution's peaked, rather than monotonically decreasing, shape indicates that the probability of a perceptual switch is low during the time period immediately following the previous switch, a temporary stability that may reflect a lack of accumulated adaptation or some other stabilizing mechanism (Kloosterman et al, 2014;Moreno-Bote et al, 2007;Pastukhov et al, 2013).…”
Section: A Note On the Distribution Of Dominance Timesmentioning
confidence: 97%
“…It has been shown to be rather similar for humans and non-human primates and together with the degree of serial correlation between dominance times it has been an important element in the debate on the relative roles of neural adaptation and neural noise in driving perceptual alternations (Fox & Herrmann, 1967;Moreno-Bote et al, 2007;Pastukhov et al, 2013;Shpiro et al, 2009;van Ee, 2009). On a general level, the distribution's peaked, rather than monotonically decreasing, shape indicates that the probability of a perceptual switch is low during the time period immediately following the previous switch, a temporary stability that may reflect a lack of accumulated adaptation or some other stabilizing mechanism (Kloosterman et al, 2014;Moreno-Bote et al, 2007;Pastukhov et al, 2013). In addition, while dominance time distributions can be quite variable between individual observers, they do correlate within observers from one paradigm to the next (Carter & Pettigrew, 2003;Shannon et al, 2011;Sheppard & Pettigrew, 2006; see also Baker & Graf, 2009b; but see Gallagher & Arnold, 2014), which is consistent with the recent identification of genetic (Shannon et al, 2011) and neurochemical factors (van Loon et al, 2013) that influence dominance durations across paradigms.…”
Section: A Note On the Distribution Of Dominance Timesmentioning
confidence: 97%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…For example, perceptual adaptation is thought to prioritize perceptual sensitivity (Kohn, 2007;Pastukhov, García-Rodríguez, et al, 2013;Theodoni, Kovács, Greenlee, & Deco, 2011;Webster, 2011). Conversely, neural persistence (Coltheart, 1980;Loftus & Irwin, 1998;Pastukhov & Braun, 2013b) and a rolling average over a longer period of perceptual history are thought to be used to minimize influence of neural noise and ensure perceptual constancy Cicchini et al, 2014;Fischer & Whitney, 2014).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…It is the ubiquity of these events that raises the question of how the visual system resolves their dynamic ambiguity. The general answer to the problem is to gather and exploit prior knowledge (Friston, Breakspear, & Deco, 2012;Gregory, 2009;Metzger, 2009;Yuille & Kersten, 2006), and this process has been well studied from both behavioral (Kristjánsson http & Campana, 2010;Pastukhov & Braun, 2011, 2013b and theoretical (Friston et al, 2012;Pastukhov, García-Rodríguez, et al, 2013) perspectives, even though the neural implementations are still poorly understood (Daw, O'Doherty, Dayan, Seymour, & Dolan, 2006). The main focus of prior research was the knowledge about physical states (Hansen, Olkkonen, Walter, & Gegenfurtner, 2006;Weiss, Simoncelli, & Adelson, 2002;Yang & Purves, 2003;Yuille & Kersten, 2006), however this type of knowledge serves only as a weak constraint because the number of transformations by far outstrips the number of states.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 98%