2016
DOI: 10.1167/16.15.17
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Motion direction influences surface segmentation in stereo transparency

Abstract: To perceive multiple overlapping surfaces in the same location of the visual field (transparency), the visual system must determine which surface elements belong together, and should be integrated, and which should be kept apart. Spatial relations between surfaces, such as depth order, must also be determined. This article details two experiments examining the interaction of motion direction and disparity cues on the perception of depth order and surface segmentation in transparency. In Experiment 1, participa… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
5

Citation Types

0
5
0

Year Published

2018
2018
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
4
2

Relationship

1
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 6 publications
(6 citation statements)
references
References 49 publications
0
5
0
Order By: Relevance
“…It has recently become clear that the perception of each observer is shaped by individual biases that can differ greatly from one observer to the next and that can persist over long periods of time (Carter & Cavanagh, 2007;Afraz, Pashkam, & Cavanagh, 2010;Mamassian & Wallace, 2010;Houlsby et al, 2013;Schütz, 2014;Wexler, Duyck, & Mamassian, 2015;Goutcher, 2016;Schütz & Mamassian, 2016;Kosovi-cheva & Whitney, 2017). For example, with my colleagues I have recently shown that the perception of two families of stimuli are shaped by individual biases that are a preferred three-dimensional (3-D) surface orientation for one of the families, and a preferred motion direction for the other family (Wexler et al, 2015).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It has recently become clear that the perception of each observer is shaped by individual biases that can differ greatly from one observer to the next and that can persist over long periods of time (Carter & Cavanagh, 2007;Afraz, Pashkam, & Cavanagh, 2010;Mamassian & Wallace, 2010;Houlsby et al, 2013;Schütz, 2014;Wexler, Duyck, & Mamassian, 2015;Goutcher, 2016;Schütz & Mamassian, 2016;Kosovi-cheva & Whitney, 2017). For example, with my colleagues I have recently shown that the perception of two families of stimuli are shaped by individual biases that are a preferred three-dimensional (3-D) surface orientation for one of the families, and a preferred motion direction for the other family (Wexler et al, 2015).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It has recently become clear that the perception of each observer is shaped by individual biases that can differ greatly from one observer to the next and that can persist over long periods of time (Carter & Cavanagh, 2007;Afraz et al, 2010;Mamassian & Wallace, 2010;Houlsby et al, 2013;Schütz, 2014;Wexler et al, 2015;Schütz & Mamassian, 2016;Goutcher, 2016;Kosovicheva & Whitney, 2017). For example, with my colleagues I have recently shown that the perception of two families of stimuli are shaped by individual biases that are a preferred 3D surface orientation for one of the families, and a preferred motion direction for the other family (Wexler et al, 2015).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Among other visual features, stereoscopic depth and motion velocity are important cues aiding segmentation. While depth (Wallace and Mamassian 2004) or motion cue alone (Wallace and Mamassian 2003) can lead to segmentation, when combined they further enhance segmentation (Snowden and Rossiter 1999;Goutcher 2016;Kohler et al 2019;Nawrot and Blake 1989;Bradshaw and Rogers 1996;Ban et al 2012). For example, psychophysical studies have shown that it becomes easier to segment overlapping surfaces moving in different directions when they are located at different depths than at the same depth (Hibbard and Bradshaw 1999;Greenwood and Edwards 2006b).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%