2018
DOI: 10.1167/18.5.7
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Multidimensional internal dynamics underlying the perception of motion

Abstract: When ambiguous visual stimuli are presented continuously, they often lead to oscillations between usually two perceptions. Because of these oscillations, it has been thought that the underlying neural dynamics also arises from a binary or two-state system. Contradicting the binary assumption, it has been shown recently that the perception of some ambiguous stimuli is governed by continuously varying internal states, measured as biases that differ considerably from one observer to the next and that can also evo… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

2
9
1

Year Published

2020
2020
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6
2

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 11 publications
(12 citation statements)
references
References 45 publications
2
9
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Our findings thus parallel the spatial heterogeneity reported for perceiving stimulus size (Afraz et al, 2010;Moutsiana et al, 2016;Schwarzkopf & Rees, 2013); location (Kosovicheva & Whitney, 2017); orientation, shape, and complex features like the apparent identity, age, or gender of faces (Afraz et al, 2010;Visconti di Oleggio Castello et al, 2018); and ambiguous motion (Wexler et al, 2015). The findings are also reminiscent of heterogeneity in the perception of bistable motion stimuli depending on their orientation (Wexler, 2018).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 83%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Our findings thus parallel the spatial heterogeneity reported for perceiving stimulus size (Afraz et al, 2010;Moutsiana et al, 2016;Schwarzkopf & Rees, 2013); location (Kosovicheva & Whitney, 2017); orientation, shape, and complex features like the apparent identity, age, or gender of faces (Afraz et al, 2010;Visconti di Oleggio Castello et al, 2018); and ambiguous motion (Wexler et al, 2015). The findings are also reminiscent of heterogeneity in the perception of bistable motion stimuli depending on their orientation (Wexler, 2018).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 83%
“…Stimuli appear smaller in peripheral compared with central vision (Anstis, 1998; Bedell & Johnson, 1984; Helmholtz, 1924; Moutsiana et al., 2016; Newsome, 1972). Even basic visual features, such as object position, size, and shape, appear differently across the visual field and viewing conditions or when measured at different times (Afraz et al., 2010; Wexler, 2018; Wexler et al., 2015). Further studies confirmed such biases for the apparent size (Moutsiana et al., 2016; Schwarzkopf & Rees, 2013) and position (Kosovicheva & Whitney, 2017) of visual stimuli.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…These findings indicate that idiosyncratic preferences in a visual feature (motion direction) can be dissociated at different stages of visual information processing. The weak directional preferences in binocular rivalry are in stark contrast to findings from other types of ambiguous stimuli (Schütz, 2014;Wexler, Duyck, & Mamassian, 2015;Wexler, 2018), suggesting that motion direction plays a less important role in binocular rivalry. Andersen and Bradley (1998) proposed that V1 and MT play an important role in the processing of structure-from-motion.…”
Section: Discussioncontrasting
confidence: 83%
“…This subjective response, like any manual response to a subjective phenomenon, includes a response bias that is entirely dependent on the viewer. A growing body of research has shown that the rate of perceptual reversals and reversal times of ambiguous figures are associated to important interindividual differences in healthy individuals [21][22][23][24]. This variability might index either the individual temporal characteristics of the perceptual alternation itself, or the individual biases related to the need to give an explicit, subjective response.…”
Section: Why Track Eye Movements?mentioning
confidence: 99%