2015
DOI: 10.1177/1368430215583519
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Moderators of the disapproval of peer punishment

Abstract: Recent studies have found disapproval of peer punishment of norm violations. This seems puzzling, given the potential benefits peer punishers contribute to the group. We suggest part of the answer is that peer punishers tend to come across as aggressive and as such may be viewed as more problematic than beneficial to have around. We used simple computer animations of geometric shapes to enact 15 precise variations of social sanctions against a norm violator. More than 1,800 subjects were recruited to watch an … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

2
43
1

Year Published

2016
2016
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
6

Relationship

0
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 43 publications
(56 citation statements)
references
References 48 publications
2
43
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Since such punishment is more costly for the enforcer but also more harmful for the transgressor, 5-yearolds may find it more challenging to judge active punishment as positively as they judged the reprimanding in the present study. Indeed, even adults seem to disapprove of severe or overly aggressive punishment of norm violators (Eriksson et al, 2016). Thus, the capacity that 5-year-olds demonstrated in our study may not generalize to more severe forms of norm enforcement.…”
Section: Discussioncontrasting
confidence: 55%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Since such punishment is more costly for the enforcer but also more harmful for the transgressor, 5-yearolds may find it more challenging to judge active punishment as positively as they judged the reprimanding in the present study. Indeed, even adults seem to disapprove of severe or overly aggressive punishment of norm violators (Eriksson et al, 2016). Thus, the capacity that 5-year-olds demonstrated in our study may not generalize to more severe forms of norm enforcement.…”
Section: Discussioncontrasting
confidence: 55%
“…A few empirical studies have examined the question of how costly norm enforcement could be sustained (e.g., Barclay, 2006;Horita, 2010;Kiyonari & Barclay, 2008;Nelissen, 2008) and have shown that enforcers do typically receive more reputational and material benefits than non-enforcers (though these effects are not unequivocal and adults may even disapprove of particularly severe or aggressive norm enforcement; Eriksson, Andersson, & Strimling, 2016). However, these studies have all involved adults, leaving unclear when in ontogeny this solution to the problem of second-order cooperation becomes functional.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A finding that disrupting the DLPFC made UG responders more accepting of unfair offers (10) seems to contradict the current findings if rejection of unfair UG offers is considered a norm-enforcing behavior. However, the latter assumption has been increasingly criticized (40,(44)(45)(46)(47)(48)(49), and the critics provide evidence that rejection of unfair offers as emotion-based aggression is against the dominant social norm, at least in highly industrialized societies (40,44,45,48,49), and is instead a long-term strategy to protect oneself from possible future exploitation by others (47,48). From this alternative view, not rejecting and accepting the disadvantageous offer is an internalized and intuitive norm-abiding behavior that must be controlled to execute strategies to protect the player's reputation and standing (47)(48)(49).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This particular form of communication is frequently referred to as "social control" (Brauer & Chekroun, 2005), but has also been called "speaking up" (Morrison & Milliken, 2000), "confronting the person" (Rattan & Dweck, 2010), "social sanctions" (Eriksson, Andersson, & Strimling, 2015), "pressure to conform" (Schachter, 1951), "altruistic punishment" (Balafoutas, Nikiforakis, & Rockenbach, 2016), "moral criticism" (Voiklis & Malle, 2016), "norm enforcement" (Horne, 2009), and "civil courage" (Greitemeyer, Osswald, Fischer, & Frey, 2007). This particular form of communication is frequently referred to as "social control" (Brauer & Chekroun, 2005), but has also been called "speaking up" (Morrison & Milliken, 2000), "confronting the person" (Rattan & Dweck, 2010), "social sanctions" (Eriksson, Andersson, & Strimling, 2015), "pressure to conform" (Schachter, 1951), "altruistic punishment" (Balafoutas, Nikiforakis, & Rockenbach, 2016), "moral criticism" (Voiklis & Malle, 2016), "norm enforcement" (Horne, 2009), and "civil courage" (Greitemeyer, Osswald, Fischer, & Frey, 2007).…”
Section: Social Controlmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…When witnessing a behavior that transgresses social norms or moral standards, bystanders sometimes communicate to the perpetrator that his or her behavior is wrong. This particular form of communication is frequently referred to as "social control" (Brauer & Chekroun, 2005), but has also been called "speaking up" (Morrison & Milliken, 2000), "confronting the person" (Rattan & Dweck, 2010), "social sanctions" (Eriksson, Andersson, & Strimling, 2015), "pressure to conform" (Schachter, 1951), "altruistic punishment" (Balafoutas, Nikiforakis, & Rockenbach, 2016), "moral criticism" (Voiklis & Malle, 2016), "norm enforcement" (Horne, 2009), and "civil courage" (Greitemeyer, Osswald, Fischer, & Frey, 2007). Social control perpetuates social norms (Gibbs, 1981) and promotes prosocial behaviors (Yamagishi, 1986).…”
Section: Social Controlmentioning
confidence: 99%