1979
DOI: 10.2466/pms.1979.48.3.807
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Modality-Specific Effects on Discrimination of Short Empty Time Intervals

Abstract: The ability of human subjects to judge the duration of short empty time intervals was studied in relation to the modality composition of the marker signals. At each trial, a pair of empty intervals was presented by a series of three successive stimuli, and the subject was asked to point out the longer interval of the two. Tone pips and flashes of light were used as the bounding signals. All the possible combinations of auditory and visual stimuli were used, in random order, to delimit pairs of intervals. Perfo… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1

Citation Types

0
9
0

Year Published

1983
1983
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 9 publications
(9 citation statements)
references
References 5 publications
0
9
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Therefore, the effect is different from other phenomena like the classical filled-duration illusion or the sharing of attention between two sources of information (Allan, 1979;Thomas & Brown, 1974;Thomas & Cantor, 1978). Indeed, this effect is more like the one reported by Hocherman and Ben-Dov (1979): when an observer discriminates among empty time intervals with some sort of forced-ehoice presentation, the observer judges the first interval to be longer more often when the observer is presented with an A-V-A sequence than when presented with a V-A-V sequence.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 50%
“…Therefore, the effect is different from other phenomena like the classical filled-duration illusion or the sharing of attention between two sources of information (Allan, 1979;Thomas & Brown, 1974;Thomas & Cantor, 1978). Indeed, this effect is more like the one reported by Hocherman and Ben-Dov (1979): when an observer discriminates among empty time intervals with some sort of forced-ehoice presentation, the observer judges the first interval to be longer more often when the observer is presented with an A-V-A sequence than when presented with a V-A-V sequence.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 50%
“…Admittedly, bimodal intervals accessing a central clock more "directly" would lead to optimal performance. Surprisingly, in duration-discrimination tasks, performance levels obtained under bimodal conditions have been shown to be much lower than those obtained with empty unimodal, auditory, or visual time intervals (Fraisse, 1952;Grondin & Rousseau, 1991;Hocherman & Ben-Dov, 1979;Rousseau, Poirier, & Lemyre, 1983). At first view, such findings can be interpreted as detrimental to the notion of a central clock concept.…”
mentioning
confidence: 72%
“…Rousseau et al argued that the similarity of slopes for bimodal and unimodal functions was an indication ofa common timekeeping mechanism operating under all interval conditions, while the differences in intercept were an indication of the contribution of nontemporal processes. Most authors have considered the lower performance level with bimodal intervals as support for mixed timing interpretations (Grondin, 1993;Grondin & Rousseau, 1991;Hocherman & Ben-Dov, 1979;Rousseau & Kristofferson, 1973).That is to say,both uni-and bimodal intervals are handled by a central timing mechanism, yet bimodal intervals are not supported by efficient modality-specific encoding processes, which would reduce the variability of the discrimination performance for unimodal intervals.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations