2007
DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2006.09.011
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Minimization of dependency length in written English

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

3
102
0

Year Published

2010
2010
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
5
3
1

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 106 publications
(108 citation statements)
references
References 19 publications
3
102
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In combination with the preference for given-new ordering, we might then expect a cross-linguistic preference for orderings in which the object follows the subject, and this has in fact been observed: The three most common orderings of subject/verb/object-V-S-O, S-O-V, and S-V-O-all place the object after the subject (Dryer, 2005). As noted earlier, it has also been observed that object phrases tend to be longer than subject phrases, at least in English (Temperley, 2007); no doubt this is partly due to the much stronger tendency of subject phrases to be given rather than new.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 85%
“…In combination with the preference for given-new ordering, we might then expect a cross-linguistic preference for orderings in which the object follows the subject, and this has in fact been observed: The three most common orderings of subject/verb/object-V-S-O, S-O-V, and S-V-O-all place the object after the subject (Dryer, 2005). As noted earlier, it has also been observed that object phrases tend to be longer than subject phrases, at least in English (Temperley, 2007); no doubt this is partly due to the much stronger tendency of subject phrases to be given rather than new.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 85%
“…In availability models, speakers favour sentence structures where more readily available words are placed earlier, to reduce the need of holding already-retrieved information in memory and to start producing early (Bock & Irwin, 1980;Bock & Warren, 1985;Branigan, Pickering, & Tanaka, 2008;Bresnan et al, 2007;Ferreira & Dell, 2000;Ferreira & Yoshita, 2003;Levelt & Maassen, 1981;Prat-Sala & Branigan, 2000;Tanaka, Branigan, McLean, & Pickering, 2011;see Jaeger & Norcliffe, 2009, for an overview). In contrast, efficiency-based theories argue that the parser prefers word orders that allow the fastest computation of constituent structure (Hawkins, 1994(Hawkins, , 2014Temperley, 2007).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This preference to produce shorter phrases before longer ones should hold regardless of the typological properties of the languages involved. Indeed, the tendency to shift long constituents over short ones (often known as heavy-NP shift) is widely attested in corpora studies in VO languages like English, German 3 , Hungarian, Greek, Polish, Finnish, Rumanian and Russian (de Marneffe, Grimm, Arnon, Kirby, & Bresnan, 2012;Hawkins, 1994;Hoffman, 1999;Kizach, 2012;Köhler, 1999;Lohse, Hawkins, & Wasow, 2004;Temperley, 2007;Uszkoreit, 1987;Uszkoreit et al, 1998;Wasow, 2002;Wiechmann & Lohmann, 2013; for an overview, see Jaeger & Norcliffe, 2009). There is also evidence from language production experiments supporting a short-before-long preference in English (Arnold et al, 2000;Marblestone, 2007;Stallings et al, 1998;Stallings & MacDonald, 2011).…”
Section: Availability-based Modelsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…These principles have been blamed for several linguistic phenomena across languages, both by traditional linguists (Hawkins, 1994) and by computational linguists (Gildea and Temperley, 2007;Temperley, 2007). For example, (1) helps explain the "late closure" or "attach low" heuristic, whereby a modifier such as a PP is more likely to attach to the closest appropriate head (Frazier, 1979;Hobbs and Bear, 1990, for example).…”
Section: Short Dependencies In Langugagementioning
confidence: 99%