2008
DOI: 10.3758/lb.36.2.149
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Microstructural analysis of conditioned and unconditioned responses to maltodextrin

Abstract: The microstructure of licking responses was analyzed to investigate the interaction between unconditioned responses to maltodextrin and the responses to flavor cues previously associated with maltodextrin. Experiment 1 demonstrated that although the consumption of maltodextrin peaked at intermediate concentrations, the mean lick cluster size showed a positive, monotonic increase with concentration. In Experiment 2, a (conditioned stimulus) CS flavor was paired with 16% maltodextrin, whereas a CS flavor was pai… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
48
0

Year Published

2010
2010
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 35 publications
(49 citation statements)
references
References 35 publications
(56 reference statements)
1
48
0
Order By: Relevance
“…However, one might question the degree to which such associations play a role in the expression of learned preferences more generally. Others have emphasized the fact that flavor cues can also associate with the post-ingestive consequences of nutrient consumption-for example, their calories or the reinforcement signal generated by processing those calories (see, e.g., Sclafani & Nissenbaum, 1988), as well as, possibly, the positive hedonic responses to highly palatable nutrients (see, e.g., Dwyer, 2008). The present experiments were not designed to address questions concerning the relative contributions of these different types of associations to the expression of preferences more generally.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…However, one might question the degree to which such associations play a role in the expression of learned preferences more generally. Others have emphasized the fact that flavor cues can also associate with the post-ingestive consequences of nutrient consumption-for example, their calories or the reinforcement signal generated by processing those calories (see, e.g., Sclafani & Nissenbaum, 1988), as well as, possibly, the positive hedonic responses to highly palatable nutrients (see, e.g., Dwyer, 2008). The present experiments were not designed to address questions concerning the relative contributions of these different types of associations to the expression of preferences more generally.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It is widely recognized that flavor preference learning can result in a variety of associations that can influence intake. For instance, when a flavor cue is paired with a nutrient, such as sucrose, the cue can enter in to an association with the specific taste of sucrose (e.g., Delamater, Campese, LoLordo & Sclafani, 2006;Dwyer, 2005;Harris et al, 2004), with the reinforcing post ingestive consequences of sucrose (e.g., Sclafani & Nissenbaum, 1988), or with the positive hedonic response to sucrose (e.g., Dwyer, 2008;Harris et al, 2004). When one pairs an initially neutral flavor cue with the oral consumption of sucrose and then demonstrates that rats prefer this flavor cue to another that has not been so paired, this preference can be explained by the operation of any one of these flavor-nutrient associations.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In addition, studies of conditioned taste aversion have also shown that palatability and consumption can dissociate (e.g. Dwyer et al 2008;Dwyer 2008). …”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Specifically, fructose has a sweet taste (there is good generalization between sucrose and fructose in rodents; Nissenbaum & Sclafani, 1987), but it is not readily metabolized; consumed orally, it will support flavor-preference learning, but intragastric infusions are much less effective (Ackroff, Touzani, Peets, & Sclafani, 2001;Sclafani et al, 1993). Maltodextrin, on the other hand, will support the conditioning of a flavor preference when it is presented by intragastric infusion , but although its taste appears to be palatable (e.g., Dwyer, 2008), there is little evidence to indicate that its taste alone would engender a conditioned preference. Elizalde and Sclafani (1988) reported that ingested maltodextrin did not support preference learning when carbohydrate digestion was blocked by a drug treatment.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%