2018
DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00903
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Metacognitive Myopia in Hidden-Profile Tasks: The Failure to Control for Repetition Biases

Abstract: The failure to exploit collective wisdom is evident in the conspicuous difficulty to solve hidden-profile tasks. While previous accounts focus on group-dynamics and motivational biases, the present research applies a metacognitive perspective to an ordinary learning approach. Assuming that evaluative learning is sensitive to the frequency with which targets are paired with positive versus negative attributes, selective repetition of targets’ assets and deficits will inevitably bias the resulting evaluations. A… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

1
20
0
1

Year Published

2019
2019
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
4
1
1

Relationship

4
2

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 15 publications
(22 citation statements)
references
References 67 publications
1
20
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…They often follow the information given uncritically, regardless of its validity. Metacognitive myopia is evident in the inability not to be influenced by selectively repeated arguments in preference learning (Unkelbach, Fiedler, & Freytag, ) or in collective decision making (Fiedler, Hofferbert & Woellert, ). People continue to be influenced by misleading information they have themselves recognized to be wrong (Fiedler, Armbruster, Nickel, Walther, & Asbeck, ) and even after full debriefing about invalid feedback (Ross, Lepper, & Hubbard, ).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…They often follow the information given uncritically, regardless of its validity. Metacognitive myopia is evident in the inability not to be influenced by selectively repeated arguments in preference learning (Unkelbach, Fiedler, & Freytag, ) or in collective decision making (Fiedler, Hofferbert & Woellert, ). People continue to be influenced by misleading information they have themselves recognized to be wrong (Fiedler, Armbruster, Nickel, Walther, & Asbeck, ) and even after full debriefing about invalid feedback (Ross, Lepper, & Hubbard, ).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The 14-fold leading status of Team A was coded as if it reflected 14 independent outcomes providing increasing evidence for the superiority of A over B. This bias is in line with previous evidence on the metacognitive inability to discount redundant information (e.g., Fiedler et al, 2018;Unkelbach et al, 2007).…”
Section: Analyzing the Task Setting Underlying The Crbmentioning
confidence: 84%
“…Prior to the analysis of the dyadic contingency judgments, the esti- decision-making (Fiedler, 2000(Fiedler, , 2012Fiedler, Hütter, Schott, & Kutzner, 2019, Fiedler, Hofferbert, & Wöllert, 2018Unkelbach et al, 2007;Juslin et al, 2007;Mahmoodi et al, 2015). Individuals' inferences often follow the given samples of information in an uncritical and naïve way.…”
Section: Dyadic Judgmentsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A growing body of evidence points to a conspicuous neglect of metacognitive monitoring and control functions (Ackerman & Thompson, 2017;Nelson, 1996). This deficit in quality control of one's own cognitive processes constitutes a serious impediment of rational thought at the individual level (Fiedler, 2000(Fiedler, , 2012Fiedler, Hütter, Schott, & Kutzner, 2019, Fiedler, Hofferbert, & Wöllert, 2018Unkelbach, Fiedler, & Freytag, 2007). The experiments reported below extend MM to dyadic groups, testing whether dyads fed with conflicting information can jointly overcome MM when they are sensitized to validity issues.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 96%
See 1 more Smart Citation