1985
DOI: 10.1080/10862968509547542
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Metacognitive Abilities of Above and below Average Readers: Effects of Conceptual Tempo, Passage Level, and Error Type on Error Detection

Abstract: A number of researchers have suggested recently that differences between good and poor comprehenders lie in differences in metacognitive skills. This study examines one of the paradigms used to measure metacognitive skill-the error detection task. Seventy-four above and below grade level sixth graders were asked to detect order changes and nonsense word substitutions in thirdgrade and sixth-grade level passages. It was found that conceptual tempo (impulsivity/reflectivity), passage readability, and error type,… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
6
0

Year Published

1986
1986
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

2
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 11 publications
(6 citation statements)
references
References 32 publications
0
6
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Not only do poor readers not know many of the words they encounter, they may not realize which words they do not know (Erickson, Stahl, & Rinehart, 1985). In addition, because they get into a pattern of skipping words that are not immediately accessible, poor readers often appear unaware that they have any partial knowledge about words and may not use what they know about partially unknown words to help with the larger context.…”
Section: For Children Having Extra Difficultymentioning
confidence: 93%
“…Not only do poor readers not know many of the words they encounter, they may not realize which words they do not know (Erickson, Stahl, & Rinehart, 1985). In addition, because they get into a pattern of skipping words that are not immediately accessible, poor readers often appear unaware that they have any partial knowledge about words and may not use what they know about partially unknown words to help with the larger context.…”
Section: For Children Having Extra Difficultymentioning
confidence: 93%
“…Although both age and reading proficiency have been linked to evaluation ability, results across studies have not been consistent. For example, although many investigators have found that older children and better readers are better able to evaluate their understanding than younger children or poor readers (Baker, 1984a(Baker, , 1984bGarner & Taylor, 1982;Markman, 1977;Winograd & Johnston, 1982), others have failed to find these developmental and reading proficiency effects under certain conditions (Erickson, Stahl, & Rinehart, 1985;Markman, 1979;Markman & Gorin, 1981;Zabrucky & Ratner, in press). And although investigators typically report that young children or poor readers are not effective at evaluating their own understanding, some researchers have found signs of evaluation ability in children as young as 5 or 6 years of age (e.g., Baker, 1984a;Flavell, Speer, Green, & August, 1981;Patterson, O'Brien, Kister, Carter, & Kotsonis, 1981) and in children identified as poor readers (Erickson, Stahl, & Rinehart, 1985;Zabrucky & Ratner, in press).…”
Section: Karen Zabruckymentioning
confidence: 95%
“…In a derivation task, the participant is told that the word is unknown or a nonsense word is substituted. In the more incidental learning of word meanings during free reading, readers are often unaware that they do not know certain words in the text (e.g., Erickson, Stahl, & Rinehart, 1985), and thus they do not apply special attention to those words. Instead, Stahl (1991) speculated that incidental learning may be explained using a connectionist metaphor.…”
Section: Deriving Words Versus Incidental Learningmentioning
confidence: 99%