2022
DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2021.104962
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Mental chronometry of speaking in dialogue: Semantic interference turns into facilitation

Help me understand this report
View preprint versions

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

0
2
0

Year Published

2023
2023
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
4
1

Relationship

0
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 5 publications
(4 citation statements)
references
References 57 publications
0
2
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In addition, all of the tasks required participants to make inferences about the mental states of anonymous others (often even nonreal avatars) as a third-party observer; these isolated contexts do not reflect the complexity and dynamic nature of real-life social interaction. Interactivity is known to alter sensitivity to others' perspectives and influence communication success (Kuhlen & Abdel Rahman, 2022; Surtees, Apperly, & Samson, 2016). Thus, future research is needed to test the extent to which findings in these controlled contexts truly reflect real-life interaction with co-present others (as in De Lillo et al, 2021), and whether older adults might particularly benefit from these interactive social contexts that motivate them to engage with others more closely (Hess, 2014).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In addition, all of the tasks required participants to make inferences about the mental states of anonymous others (often even nonreal avatars) as a third-party observer; these isolated contexts do not reflect the complexity and dynamic nature of real-life social interaction. Interactivity is known to alter sensitivity to others' perspectives and influence communication success (Kuhlen & Abdel Rahman, 2022; Surtees, Apperly, & Samson, 2016). Thus, future research is needed to test the extent to which findings in these controlled contexts truly reflect real-life interaction with co-present others (as in De Lillo et al, 2021), and whether older adults might particularly benefit from these interactive social contexts that motivate them to engage with others more closely (Hess, 2014).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, when their task is to produce one utterance, they may inhibit their simulation of another utteranceaccounting for the undetailed nature of co-representation when participants are concurrently engaged in a language production task. Such inhibition of detailed co-representation may be benefical in communicative contexts, where it may help facilitate conceptual processing (see Kuhlen & Abdel Rahman, 2022).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, this reduction in the magnitude of the semantic interference effect only occurred when the distractor words were presented in case-alternating font, suggesting that representation of the partner's utterances can sometimes help participants ignore distracting information that is task-relevant for the partner, but only when processing of that information is made less automatic (in contrast, when the words were presented in regular font, or when participants believed their partner was naming the color of the pictures, comparable levels of interference were found in joint and individual versions). Finally, Kuhlen and Abdel Rahman (2022) found that when the PWI task is embedded in a communicative game, with one participant naming the distractor words and the other, co-present, participant naming the pictures, semantic interference is also greatly reduced (compared to a non-communicative, standard version of the PWI task). They suggested that naming pictures in a communicative setting may enhance semantic 13 facilitation at the conceptual level (due to distractor and target belonging to the same semantic category).…”
Section: Joint Stroop and Joint Picture-word Interference Tasksmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In addition, it is essential for assessing the scope of psycholinguistic processing models of speaking and listening. These models are largely based on experimental work carried out in laboratory environments, which differ in many ways from the environments where conversations are typically held ( Kandylaki & Bornkessel-Schlesewsky, 2019 ; Kuhlen, Bogler, Brennan, & Haynes, 2017 ; Kuhlen & Rahman, 2022 ; Sjerps, Decuyper, & Meyer, 2019 ; Verga & Kotz, 2019 ). For instance, rather than conversing with another person, participants in lab experiments are typically tested individually, and they produce utterances in monologues or respond to recorded utterances.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%