2014
DOI: 10.1558/jch.v1i1.98
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Memory and Early Monastic Literary Practices

Abstract: This article argues that certain important aspects of the institutionalized literary practices of early cenobitic monasticism and the rhetorics related to them may be significantly illuminated by insights from the cognitive study of the human mind and its relationship with the world. Using examples from our sources of early cenobitic monasticism in Egypt, specifically writings from the Pachomian federation and Shenoute of Atripe, this article suggests ways i which cognitive perspectives on memory and literatur… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

0
2
0

Year Published

2018
2018
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
4
3

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 8 publications
(2 citation statements)
references
References 23 publications
0
2
0
Order By: Relevance
“…These texts were part of the cognitive ecology of the monks. 111 In a similar way, Malcolm Choat argues that monastic genealogies found in inscriptions in monasteries were important cognitive artefacts: 'literally embedded in the walls and floors of their cells, before their eyes every day as they walked throughout the monastery, prayed, ate, and worked, they are a much more direct and constant presence than the sermons of their abbots, or the texts they read or memorised as part of their education'. 112 Although the observations made by Lundhaug and Choat are certainly interesting, critics of historical cognitive science have pointed out that they do not see what this new theoretical framework of cognitive ecology adds to what historians have already drawn from the primary sources themselves.…”
Section: Remembering and Forgetting: A Cognitive Approachmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…These texts were part of the cognitive ecology of the monks. 111 In a similar way, Malcolm Choat argues that monastic genealogies found in inscriptions in monasteries were important cognitive artefacts: 'literally embedded in the walls and floors of their cells, before their eyes every day as they walked throughout the monastery, prayed, ate, and worked, they are a much more direct and constant presence than the sermons of their abbots, or the texts they read or memorised as part of their education'. 112 Although the observations made by Lundhaug and Choat are certainly interesting, critics of historical cognitive science have pointed out that they do not see what this new theoretical framework of cognitive ecology adds to what historians have already drawn from the primary sources themselves.…”
Section: Remembering and Forgetting: A Cognitive Approachmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This is known as distributed cognition, and it has been demonstrated how the concept can explain complex teamwork and various other kinds of interaction between an agent and his or her surroundings. Examples are the use of a notebook to aid the memory (Clark and Chalmers 2010, 33-37), the various coordinated operations necessary for running a theatre performance in the Globe (Tribble 2005), the performance of a research project (Giere and Moffatt 2003: 305-8), and negotiation and formation of memory in a social group (Barnier et al 2008;Lundhaug 2014). Distributed cognition offers interesting perspectives when discussing how inscriptions from the same context relate to each other, though it should be remarked that the concept is developed for describing simultaneous interaction and not interaction over time.…”
Section: Cognitive Theorymentioning
confidence: 99%