2017
DOI: 10.1016/j.jesp.2016.10.004
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Mechanisms underlying approach-avoidance instruction effects on implicit evaluation: Results of a preregistered adversarial collaboration

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

2
27
0

Year Published

2017
2017
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
6
3

Relationship

3
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 42 publications
(29 citation statements)
references
References 54 publications
2
27
0
Order By: Relevance
“…As a result, there might be more associative transfer of positive valence to stimulus representations when participants receive self-agent approach instructions than when they receive stimulus-agent instructions. It should be noted, however, that a recent study provided little evidence for the involvement of representations of the self in AA instruction effects (Van Dessel, Gawronski, Smith, & De Houwer, 2017). It is, however, also possible that self-agent action representations are more positive because they have stronger associations with other valenced representations (e.g., representations of motivational systems of approach and avoidance, Neumann, Förster, & Strack, 2003), which facilitates associative transfer of valence in the context of self-agent instructions.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 96%
“…As a result, there might be more associative transfer of positive valence to stimulus representations when participants receive self-agent approach instructions than when they receive stimulus-agent instructions. It should be noted, however, that a recent study provided little evidence for the involvement of representations of the self in AA instruction effects (Van Dessel, Gawronski, Smith, & De Houwer, 2017). It is, however, also possible that self-agent action representations are more positive because they have stronger associations with other valenced representations (e.g., representations of motivational systems of approach and avoidance, Neumann, Förster, & Strack, 2003), which facilitates associative transfer of valence in the context of self-agent instructions.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 96%
“…Prior studies have demonstrated the effectiveness of outcome expectancies induced by verbal suggestions in relieving itch and pain in healthy participants [ 21 , 22 ], and have shown that verbal suggestions are able to induce analgesic effects in various clinical patient populations, including patients with irritable bowel syndrome and patients undergoing thoracotomy [ 23 25 ]. A second way to influence health outcomes is by means of verbal suggestions that influence specific actions of approaching and avoiding certain stimuli without actually performing these actions, i.e., stimulus-response contingency instructions [ 26 29 ]. Verbal suggestions concerning stimulus-response contingencies were recently shown to alter evaluations of fictitious social groups or meaningless words [ 27 29 ].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A second way to influence health outcomes is by means of verbal suggestions that influence specific actions of approaching and avoiding certain stimuli without actually performing these actions, i.e., stimulus-response contingency instructions [ 26 29 ]. Verbal suggestions concerning stimulus-response contingencies were recently shown to alter evaluations of fictitious social groups or meaningless words [ 27 29 ]. These findings suggest that the effectiveness of gamified approach-avoidance trainings might be strengthened by verbal suggestions.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Recent studies, however, have challenged the idea that evaluative effects are underlain by associations formed through repeated practice (see De Houwer, Van Dessel, & Moran, in press, & Corneille & Stahl, 2019, for reviews). An important observation in this debate is that evaluative effects can also be induced on the basis of mere instructions describing contingencies of stimuli and actions, without overtly practicing these contingencies (Van Dessel, De Houwer, Gast, Smith, & De Schryver, 2016;Van Dessel, Gawronski, Smith, & De Houwer, 2017). For instance, Van Dessel, De Houwer, Gast, and Smith (2015) instructed participants to approach one non-word and avoid another non-word.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%