1989
DOI: 10.1068/p180303
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Measuring Attention Using Induced Motion

Abstract: Attention was measured by means of its effect upon induced motion. Perceived horizontal motion was induced in a vertically moving test spot by the physical horizontal motion of inducing objects. All stimuli were in a frontoparallel plane. The induced motion vectored with the physical motion to produce a clockwise or counterclockwise tilt in the apparent path of motion of the test spot. Either a single inducing object or two inducing objects moving in opposite directions were used. Twelve observers were instruc… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
12
0

Year Published

1990
1990
2015
2015

Publication Types

Select...
10

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 57 publications
(14 citation statements)
references
References 32 publications
2
12
0
Order By: Relevance
“…serve as a unifying explanation for other cases that seem at first to defy these categories. For example, the direction and magnitude of 1M is influenced by attention to competing inducing stimuli (Gogel & MacCracken, 1979;Gogel & Sharkey, 1989;Gogel & Tietz, 1976). This is consistent with enhancement of OKN gain by attention directed at a moving display (Barnes & Hill, 1984;Cheng & Outerbridge, 1975;Dubois & Collewijn, 1979;HolmJensen, 1984).…”
Section: Resultssupporting
confidence: 58%
“…serve as a unifying explanation for other cases that seem at first to defy these categories. For example, the direction and magnitude of 1M is influenced by attention to competing inducing stimuli (Gogel & MacCracken, 1979;Gogel & Sharkey, 1989;Gogel & Tietz, 1976). This is consistent with enhancement of OKN gain by attention directed at a moving display (Barnes & Hill, 1984;Cheng & Outerbridge, 1975;Dubois & Collewijn, 1979;HolmJensen, 1984).…”
Section: Resultssupporting
confidence: 58%
“…The other is capable of producing size and shape constancy or, more generally, is consistent with the requirements of the size-distance and shape-slant invariance hypotheses (Boring, 1946;Epstein & Babler, 1989;Epstein & Broota, 1986;Epstein & Lovitts, 1985;Holway & Boring, 1941;Mack, 1978;Rock, 1977Rock, , 1983. In terms of the theory presented in this paper, a seeming regression toward the proximal stimulus and, thus, toward independence from perceived distance as cue effectiveness is lessened either by cue reduction or by withdrawal of attention is misleading (Gogel & DaSilva, 1987b;Gogel & Sharkey, 1989). Instead of indicating independence from perceived distance, such results indicate only that the stimuli are perceived as displaced toward a constant perceived distance defined by the specific distance tendency and/or the equidistance tendency.…”
Section: Phenomenal Geometrymentioning
confidence: 94%
“…If the subjects were attending to different features of the stimulus in the different conditions, then the results may merely reflect differences in people's abilities to encode particular features of a visual stimulus, and not necessarily differences in the type of information available for localization. There is ample evidence that attentional factors can affect visual illusions, and it is important to determine whether such factors contributed to the present findings (Coren & Porac, 1983;Gogel & Sharkey, 1988;Kelso, Cook, Olson, & Epstein, 1975;Tsal, 1984).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%