2003
DOI: 10.1016/s0376-6357(02)00124-9
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Matching unrelated stimuli with same discriminative functions: training order effects

Abstract: Previous research has shown that after training simple discriminations (A1+/A2-, B1+/B2-), bringing these tasks under conditional control (J1-A1, J2-A2) leads to transfer of discriminative control (J1+/J2-) and to generalized matching on the basis of same discriminative functions (e.g. J1-B1, J2-B2). The same occurs when conditional discriminations are trained (D1-E1, D2-E2; F1-G1, F2-G2). When the subjects are then trained to demonstrate correct relations (D1-E1, D2-E2) when given X1 and to demonstrate incorr… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1

Citation Types

0
7
0

Year Published

2004
2004
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6

Relationship

2
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 8 publications
(7 citation statements)
references
References 21 publications
0
7
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The studies by Carpentier et al (2003) and by Serna and PerezGonzalez (2003) showed that the functions of contextual stimuli may not be acquired or transferred under certain conditions. The main goal of the present research was to further study the conditions necessary for acquiring contextual control of specific functions and for transferring this control to novel conditional discriminations.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The studies by Carpentier et al (2003) and by Serna and PerezGonzalez (2003) showed that the functions of contextual stimuli may not be acquired or transferred under certain conditions. The main goal of the present research was to further study the conditions necessary for acquiring contextual control of specific functions and for transferring this control to novel conditional discriminations.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…These studies, however, did not analyze the conditions under which such a process occurs. In contrast, the study by Carpentier et al (2003) included one condition in which the second-order conditional discrimination was taught before the first-order conditional discrimination. Although all 6 participants showed transfer of contextual stimulus functions in the original condition (i.e., when the participants learned the first-order conditional discriminations first) , only 4 of 6 participants showed transfer of contextual stimulus functions when they learned the second-order conditional discrimination first.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…After being trained on conditional A-B and A-C discrimination tasks and on a simple O-discrimination task (01+/02-), most subjects (a) selected BC compounds with same-class elements in a BC discrimination test (e.g., B1C1+/B1C2-), and (b) matched BC compounds with other BC compounds and with unitary 0 stimuli of same discriminative functions (e.g., B1C1-B3C3, B1C2-B2C3; 01-B3C3, 02-B2C3) . Because (a) the BC-Face (Carpentier et aI., 2003b) and O-BC performances (Carpentier et aI., 2004) could have resulted only from matching same discriminative functions, and (b) previous studies have shown that adults and children readily relate stimuli to any other stimuli of same discriminative functions (Carpentier, Smeets, & Barnes-Holmes, 2002a, 2002c, 2003aPerez-Gonzalez, 1994;Perez-Gonzalez & Serna, 2003), it should be assumed that the BC-BC performances were based on the same process.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In a similar vein, a recent study in our laboratory arranged simple successive discrimination contingencies in which sets of three stimuli were correlated with reinforcement for the same response and documented the development of equivalence relations among those stimuli in unreinforced probe trials (Vaidya, Maciver, & Eiliefsen, in prep). Other studies (de Rose et al 1988;Carpentier et al 2003) have similarly shown that three-term contingencies are sufficient to give rise to equivalence relations among stimuli.…”
mentioning
confidence: 83%