The platform will undergo maintenance on Sep 14 at about 7:45 AM EST and will be unavailable for approximately 2 hours.
2021
DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0258470
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Mask exposure during COVID-19 changes emotional face processing

Abstract: Faces are one of the key ways that we obtain social information about others. They allow people to identify individuals, understand conversational cues, and make judgements about others’ mental states. When the COVID-19 pandemic hit the United States, widespread mask-wearing practices were implemented, causing a shift in the way Americans typically interact. This introduction of masks into social exchanges posed a potential challenge—how would people make these important inferences about others when a large so… Show more

Help me understand this report
View preprint versions

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

5
25
0
2

Year Published

2021
2021
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
9
1

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 35 publications
(32 citation statements)
references
References 86 publications
5
25
0
2
Order By: Relevance
“…These are considered crucial areas for facial emotion recognition, mainly disgust and happiness [ 19 ]; therefore, the use of masks may lead to inaccurate face processing which can crucially affect social everyday interactions [ 20 , 21 , 22 , 23 ]. Detailed processing of human faces is already more difficult with partial occlusions commonly used in everyday life (i.e., sunglasses, scarfs, headdresses, veils) [ 24 , 25 , 26 ]; consequently, it might be even more altered when people wear masks that cover between 60% and 70% of the entire face, depending on shapes and sizes [ 27 ], forcing people to ground the emotion identification mainly on cues from the eyes [ 28 ]. Some studies have been conducted in healthy controls on this topic since the beginning of the COVID-19 pandemic, highlighting the lower accuracy in the emotion recognition of faces wearing masks [ 27 , 29 , 30 , 31 ].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…These are considered crucial areas for facial emotion recognition, mainly disgust and happiness [ 19 ]; therefore, the use of masks may lead to inaccurate face processing which can crucially affect social everyday interactions [ 20 , 21 , 22 , 23 ]. Detailed processing of human faces is already more difficult with partial occlusions commonly used in everyday life (i.e., sunglasses, scarfs, headdresses, veils) [ 24 , 25 , 26 ]; consequently, it might be even more altered when people wear masks that cover between 60% and 70% of the entire face, depending on shapes and sizes [ 27 ], forcing people to ground the emotion identification mainly on cues from the eyes [ 28 ]. Some studies have been conducted in healthy controls on this topic since the beginning of the COVID-19 pandemic, highlighting the lower accuracy in the emotion recognition of faces wearing masks [ 27 , 29 , 30 , 31 ].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Thus, when encountering a facial expression of forward facing, direct eye-gaze fear, the typically encountered signature of that expression is less cogent, leading to diminished detection accuracy (Adams & Kleck, 2003;Im et al, 2017;Sato et al, 2004). The effects of introducing noise to facial expression decoding can also be seen in recent research that suggests wearing a mask (hiding signal from the mouth) diminishes individuals' reliance on arousal cues (Barrick et al, 2020). Future research should investigate how obfuscation of face parts, gaze, and head positioning influence accuracy of emotional expression identification when only low or high spatial frequency information is available.…”
Section: Limitations and Future Directionsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“… Adolphs et al, 2005 , Swaminathan and & Meera, 2020 , Teo, 2021 , Veluri, 2020 , Wang, 2020 , Wong et al, 2013 , Zhang et al, 2018 , Bryce et al, 2008 , Adolphs et al, 1994 , Caseras et al, 2007 , Chaby et al, 2015 , Damer, 2020 , diAltobrando et al, 2020 , Eizenman et al, 2003 , Ferrari et al, 2016 , Asadi et al, 2020 , Forgie et al, 2009 , Galea et al, 2020 , Green et al, 2021 , Hao et al, 2020 , Hayirli et al, 2021 , Howard, 2021 , Imai and & Furukawa, 2021 , Kellough et al, 2008 , Kohler et al, 2009 , Laguesse and & Rossion, 2013 , Barrick et al, 2021 , Loey et al, 2021 , Opitz et al, 2016 , Mehta et al, 2020 , Molnar-Szakacs et al, 2021 , Schwaninger et al, 2006 , Schyns et al, 2009 …”
Section: Uncited Referencesmentioning
confidence: 99%