2016
DOI: 10.3389/fnins.2016.00533
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Lexical Specification of Prosodic Information in Swedish: Evidence from Mismatch Negativity

Abstract: Like that of many other Germanic languages, the stress system of Swedish has mainly undergone phonological analysis. Recently, however, researchers have begun to recognize the central role of morphology in these systems. Similar to the lexical specification of tonal accent, the Swedish stress system is claimed to be morphologically determined and morphemes are thus categorized as prosodically specified and prosodically unspecified. Prosodically specified morphemes bear stress information as part of their lexic… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

0
5
0

Year Published

2019
2019
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
4
2
1

Relationship

2
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 7 publications
(5 citation statements)
references
References 24 publications
(44 reference statements)
0
5
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In other words, repeatedly presenting the same lexical item in the standard position, led to more specific anticipations and activations of lexical candidates, which, in turn, resulted in mmns reflecting the concurrent detection of several deviances: (1) the acoustic deviance, (2) the phonological mismatch and, possibly, (3) the mismatch to the lexical prediction (see e.g. Pulvermüller & Shtyrov, 2006; Jacobsen et al, 2004; Honbolygó et al, 2004; Honbolygó & Csépe, 2013; Honbolygó et al, 2017; Ylinen et al, 2009; Garami et al, 2017; Zora et al, 2016, for oddball studies investigating obstructed processing due to mismatching stress templates on words and/or pseudowords in Hungarian, Finnish and Swedish).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In other words, repeatedly presenting the same lexical item in the standard position, led to more specific anticipations and activations of lexical candidates, which, in turn, resulted in mmns reflecting the concurrent detection of several deviances: (1) the acoustic deviance, (2) the phonological mismatch and, possibly, (3) the mismatch to the lexical prediction (see e.g. Pulvermüller & Shtyrov, 2006; Jacobsen et al, 2004; Honbolygó et al, 2004; Honbolygó & Csépe, 2013; Honbolygó et al, 2017; Ylinen et al, 2009; Garami et al, 2017; Zora et al, 2016, for oddball studies investigating obstructed processing due to mismatching stress templates on words and/or pseudowords in Hungarian, Finnish and Swedish).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The MMN response was even documented for the relevance of prosody in early morphological processing in Swedish, and specification of stress (lexical vs. phonological) was demonstrated to influence the processing of derivations in the brain ( Zora et al, 2019 ). These MMN results, moving beyond the signal-based perception, pinpoint that linguistic prosody is indeed accommodated in the long-term memory representations ( Honbolygó and Csépe, 2013 ; Zora et al, 2015 , 2016a , b , 2019 , 2020 ). Affective prosody was also found to modulate the amplitude of MMN response, being larger for emotional than for neutral vocalizations ( Schirmer et al, 2005 , 2007 ; Schirmer and Kotz, 2006 ; Schirmer and Escoffier, 2010 ).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 90%
“…The MMN component is based on an oddball paradigm, where a deviant stimulus is interspersed among frequent standard stimuli, and signals the brain’s automatic reaction to deviations in auditory input at around 100–250 ms after divergence point and with a fronto-central scalp distribution ( Näätänen et al, 1978 , 2007 ). The MMN response successfully indicates neural correlates of both low-level acoustic and high-level cognitive processing associated with prosodic information (e.g., Näätänen et al, 1978 ; Honbolygó et al, 2004 , 2020 ; Weber et al, 2004 ; Friederici et al, 2007 ; Zora et al, 2015 , 2016a , b , 2019 , 2020 ; Garami et al, 2017 ). Previous research indicated MMN activation to linguistic prosody change in Swedish words that are distinguished on the sole basis of pitch accent, fasen 1 “the phase” and fasen 2 “expletive,” reflecting the activation of different lexical items in the brain based on prosody ( Zora et al, 2020 ).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 93%
“…The MMN response is typically investigated using a passive oddball paradigm, where a rare stimulus (deviant) is interspersed among frequent stimuli (standard), and is elicited even when attention is directed elsewhere ( Näätänen et al, 1978 , 2007 ; Paavilainen et al, 2007 ; Winkler, 2007 ). The MMN response is optimal for investigating assimilatory processes at the pre-lexical and lexical levels, as it reflects not only low-level acoustic processing but also higher-level cognitive and linguistic processes such as activation and formation of long-term memory representations and predictive processes ( Pulvermüller et al, 2001 ; Ylinen et al, 2010 , 2016 ; Zora et al, 2015 , 2016a , b , 2019 ; Garami et al, 2017 ). Given that the amplitude of MMN depends on the degree of variance between the stimuli ( Sams et al, 1985 ; Pakarinen et al, 2007 ), several studies used the MMN component to investigate the variations introduced by assimilatory processes and their consequences for the auditory neural activity ( Mitterer and Blomert, 2003 ; Mitterer et al, 2006 ; Tavabi et al, 2009 ).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%