2018
DOI: 10.1016/j.bandl.2018.02.001
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Lexical olfaction recruits olfactory orbitofrontal cortex in metaphorical and literal contexts

Abstract: The investigation of specific lexical categories has substantially contributed to advancing our knowledge on how meaning is neurally represented. One sensory domain that has received particularly little attention is olfaction. This study aims to investigate the neural representation of lexical olfaction. In an fMRI experiment, participants read olfactory metaphors, their literal paraphrases, and literal olfactory sentences. Regions of interest were defined by a functional localizer run of odor processing. We o… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
4
1

Citation Types

2
31
1

Year Published

2019
2019
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
4
2

Relationship

0
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 34 publications
(34 citation statements)
references
References 61 publications
(47 reference statements)
2
31
1
Order By: Relevance
“…The results reviewed here suggest that mental simulation related to the lower senses may be difficult, and less common than for the "higher" senses of vision and audition (e.g., Luzzi et al, 2007;Pecher et al, 2003;Speed & Majid, 2018a;van Dantzig et al, 2008). The evidence for overlap in brain activation for sensory language and perception is tenuous at best (e.g., Goldberg et al, 2006aGoldberg et al, , 2006bPomp et al, 2018), with most studies unable to rule out the engagement of strategic mental imagery in their tasks. While there is stronger evidence for the activation of secondary sensory regions for taste and smell language, such activation suggests a different level of specificity than activation of primary olfactory and gustatory regions.…”
Section: Assessing the Evidencementioning
confidence: 77%
See 4 more Smart Citations
“…The results reviewed here suggest that mental simulation related to the lower senses may be difficult, and less common than for the "higher" senses of vision and audition (e.g., Luzzi et al, 2007;Pecher et al, 2003;Speed & Majid, 2018a;van Dantzig et al, 2008). The evidence for overlap in brain activation for sensory language and perception is tenuous at best (e.g., Goldberg et al, 2006aGoldberg et al, , 2006bPomp et al, 2018), with most studies unable to rule out the engagement of strategic mental imagery in their tasks. While there is stronger evidence for the activation of secondary sensory regions for taste and smell language, such activation suggests a different level of specificity than activation of primary olfactory and gustatory regions.…”
Section: Assessing the Evidencementioning
confidence: 77%
“…The remaining 27 sentences explicitly refer to an odour source (e.g.,"garlic" in Es ist schlecht für die Karriere, wenn man immer nach Knoblauch riecht; "It's bad for a career, always smelling of garlic."). Pomp et al (2018) suggest the lack of piriform activation is due to the overall hedonicity of the olfactory sentences-which highlight a pleasant or unpleasant odour experience-and therefore activate secondary olfactory regions where odour valence is processed. However, since valence is thought to be the primary dimension by which odours are encoded (Khan et al, 2007;Yeshurun & Sobel, 2010;Zarzo, 2008;although see Olofsson et al, 2012;Olofsson, Bowman, & Gottfried, 2013), and odour language is also strongly encoded along this dimension (Levinson & Majid, 2014;Winter, 2016;Wnuk & Majid, 2014) piriform activation ought to be expected on a fully grounded perspective.…”
Section: Olfactionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 3 more Smart Citations