1974
DOI: 10.3758/bf03336725
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Letter identification in word, nonword, and single-letter displays

Abstract: Letter identification in four-letter words, four-letter nonwords, and single-letter displays was tested using a two-alternative, forced-choice procedure. The nonwords were constructed to be pronounceable and orthographically regular. All displays were tachistoscopically presented without pre-or post exposure masking fields. The mean proportions of correct choices were .749 for letters, .735 for words, and .671 for nonwords, with the latter proportion being significantly smaller than those for the other conditi… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

4
26
0

Year Published

1977
1977
1994
1994

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 50 publications
(30 citation statements)
references
References 11 publications
4
26
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Accordingly, this particular perceptual inference model cannot easily account for the frequently reported finding that letters embedded in words are identified better than those in orthographically regular pseudowords (Juola, Leavitt, & Choe, 1974;Manelis, 1974;McClelland, 1976;Spoehr & Smith, 1973;Paap et al, Note 1). More important, there is a growing body of literature that suggests that orthographic structure does not always contribute to the magnitude of the WSE (Johnston, 1978;Manelis, 1974;McClelland & Johnston, 1977;Paap et al, Note 1).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 90%
“…Accordingly, this particular perceptual inference model cannot easily account for the frequently reported finding that letters embedded in words are identified better than those in orthographically regular pseudowords (Juola, Leavitt, & Choe, 1974;Manelis, 1974;McClelland, 1976;Spoehr & Smith, 1973;Paap et al, Note 1). More important, there is a growing body of literature that suggests that orthographic structure does not always contribute to the magnitude of the WSE (Johnston, 1978;Manelis, 1974;McClelland & Johnston, 1977;Paap et al, Note 1).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 90%
“…They replicated Reicher's word-letter effect with a pattern mask, but found performance equivalent for words and letters when the pattern mask was replaced by a contourless masking field. Juola, Leavitt, and Choe (1974) also found equivalent performance for words and letters in the Reicher task when they used a dark postexposure field. The primary question raised by these findings is why different masks differentially favor the recognition of words and letters.…”
mentioning
confidence: 87%
“…When perceptual recognition of words and pseudowords is compared, an advantage for words is frequently found (Juola, Leavitt, & Choe, 1974;Manelis, 1974;McClelland, 1976). This "word superiority effect" has sometimes been invoked as evidence for whole-word perceptual units (Juola et al, 1974;Bruder, Note 1). However, the word superiority effect could have a number of loci in addition to the hypothesized use of whole-word units in early visual processing.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%