2011
DOI: 10.1038/nn.2748
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Learning the microstructure of successful behavior

Abstract: Reinforcement signals indicating success or failure are known to alter the probability of selecting between distinct actions. However, successful performance of many motor skills, such as speech articulation, also requires learning behavioral trajectories that vary continuously over time. Here, we investigated how temporally discrete reinforcement signals shape a continuous behavioral trajectory, the fundamental frequency of adult Bengalese finch song. We provided reinforcement contingent on fundamental freque… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

7
80
0

Year Published

2012
2012
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 64 publications
(87 citation statements)
references
References 47 publications
7
80
0
Order By: Relevance
“…4A, Right) results in less reinforcement. This account of our results is consistent with recent studies that use white noisebased negative reinforcement signals to drive adaptive changes in pitch (10,24). These studies show that when a subset of pitches is punished by an aversive white noise stimulus, songbirds change their pitch distribution to avoid white noise.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 92%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…4A, Right) results in less reinforcement. This account of our results is consistent with recent studies that use white noisebased negative reinforcement signals to drive adaptive changes in pitch (10,24). These studies show that when a subset of pitches is punished by an aversive white noise stimulus, songbirds change their pitch distribution to avoid white noise.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 92%
“…In white noise experiments, a successful outcome would be an escape from negative reinforcement. Indeed, experimental conditions in which no trials are successful (very large pitch shifts in our study or previously published experiments in which white noise is provided on every trial) produce zero or near-zero learning (10,24). Therefore, despite the methodological differences between shifting the pitch of feedback and differentially reinforcing some syllable variants with white noise, we believe that learning in both paradigms can be explained by birds evaluating the relative value of a range of vocal pitches.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 46%
“…In adults, recent work indicates that the song variability continues to be significant for song plasticity. Trial-by-trial variability in adult song can enable reinforcement learning, and the degree of variability can constrain the amount of plasticity (Tumer and Brainard, 2007;Andalman and Fee, 2009;Charlesworth et al, 2011;Warren et al, 2011;Sober and Brainard, 2012).…”
Section: Trial-and-error: the Role Of Variability In Song Learning Anmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Continuous activity in HVC (RA) neurons also figures into recent models of sequence generation in which these neurons form a synaptically connected chain that supports the propagation of bursting activity (Glaze and Troyer, 2007;Hanuschkin et al, 2011;Jin et al, 2007;Li and Greenside, 2006;Long and Fee, 2008;Long et al, 2010). Finally, a recent model of vocal learning incorporates X-projecting HVC (HVC (X) ) neurons to form a continuous-time code (or clock), allowing temporal specificity in learning (Charlesworth et al, 2011;Fee and Goldberg, 2011;Ravbar et al, 2012).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%