2017
DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2016.10.019
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Learning language from within: Children use semantic generalizations to infer word meanings

Abstract: One reason that word learning presents a challenge for children is because pairings between word forms and meanings are arbitrary conventions that children must learn via observation - e.g., the fact that "shovel" labels shovels. The present studies explore cases in which children might bypass observational learning and spontaneously infer new word meanings: By exploiting the fact that many words are flexible and systematically encode multiple, related meanings. For example, words like shovel and hammer are no… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

7
37
0
1

Year Published

2017
2017
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

3
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 26 publications
(45 citation statements)
references
References 67 publications
7
37
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Consistent with this, prior studies suggest that -in contrast to the case of homophones -children do not have difficulty establishing reference for a new, polysemous use of a word (Rabagliati, Marcus, & Pylkkänen, 2010;Srinivasan et al, 2017;Srinivasan & Snedeker, 2011, 2014. For example, upon learning a novel word for an animal (e.g., that 'tima' labels a chicken), four-and five-yearolds readily shift their interpretation of the label to refer to the meat derived from the animal (e.g., such that 'tima' labels chicken meat; Srinivasan & Snedeker, 2014), and preschoolers are similarly flexible at shifting new words between representational objects and their abstract content (e.g., 'heavy book' vs. 'interesting book'; Srinivasan & Snedeker, 2011), physical containers and their contents (e.g., 'wash the pot', 'stir the pot'; Rabagliati et al, 2010), actions and tools enabling those actions (e.g., 'She shoveled the snow' vs. 'She picked up a shovel'; Srinivasan et al, 2017), and more. Strikingly, children are not only able to understand new uses of polysemous words when presented with them, but also spontaneously anticipate the different ways that polysemous words can be used to refer (Srinivasan et al, 2017).…”
Section: Identifying the Referents Of New Homophonous And Polysemous supporting
confidence: 55%
See 3 more Smart Citations
“…Consistent with this, prior studies suggest that -in contrast to the case of homophones -children do not have difficulty establishing reference for a new, polysemous use of a word (Rabagliati, Marcus, & Pylkkänen, 2010;Srinivasan et al, 2017;Srinivasan & Snedeker, 2011, 2014. For example, upon learning a novel word for an animal (e.g., that 'tima' labels a chicken), four-and five-yearolds readily shift their interpretation of the label to refer to the meat derived from the animal (e.g., such that 'tima' labels chicken meat; Srinivasan & Snedeker, 2014), and preschoolers are similarly flexible at shifting new words between representational objects and their abstract content (e.g., 'heavy book' vs. 'interesting book'; Srinivasan & Snedeker, 2011), physical containers and their contents (e.g., 'wash the pot', 'stir the pot'; Rabagliati et al, 2010), actions and tools enabling those actions (e.g., 'She shoveled the snow' vs. 'She picked up a shovel'; Srinivasan et al, 2017), and more. Strikingly, children are not only able to understand new uses of polysemous words when presented with them, but also spontaneously anticipate the different ways that polysemous words can be used to refer (Srinivasan et al, 2017).…”
Section: Identifying the Referents Of New Homophonous And Polysemous supporting
confidence: 55%
“…Instead, the most prevalent form of ambiguity in natural language is polysemy -cases in which the same word expresses multiple, related senses (e.g., 'tin' and 'glass' can each label materials or objects made from those materials). Critically, in contrast to homophones, which express unrelated meanings, polysemous senses are related, raising the possibility that children's knowledge of one sense of a polysemous word might actually facilitate -rather than hinder -their acquisition of other polysemous senses (Rabagliati & Srinivasan, in press; Srinivasan, Al-Mughairy, Foushee, & Barner, 2017;Srinivasan & Rabagliati, 2015).…”
Section: Children's Use Of Polysemy To Structure New Word Meaningsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Verder word daar ook internasionaal heelwat vergelykende studies uitgevoer van eerstetaalverwerwing in verskillende tale (vergelyk Bassano & Hickmann 2013;Kail & Hickmann 2010;Küntay et al 2014). Beskrywende studies waarin ondersoek ingestel word na fonologiese (Archer & Curtin 2016;Kaur & Rau 2015;Swingley 2017;Tribushinina 2013), semantiese (Abend et al 2017;Srinivasan et al 2017) en morfosintaktiese (Cuza 2016;Legendre et al 2014;Lustigman 2013) …”
Section: Eerstetaalverwerwingnavorsing Vanuit 'N Taalkundige Perspektiefunclassified