2021
DOI: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2021.101075
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Preschoolers’ acquisition of producer-product metonymy

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

2
12
0

Year Published

2022
2022
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
5
1
1

Relationship

3
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 8 publications
(18 citation statements)
references
References 39 publications
2
12
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Overall, the present experiment contributes to a growing body of literature suggesting that preschoolers may possess a relatively sophisticated ability to understand and use nonliteral language (Falkum et al, 2017;Pouscoulous & Tomasello, 2020;Zhu, 2021;Zhu et al, 2020). Moreover, by demonstrating that preschoolers can make additional inferences from novel metaphors, the present research suggests that metaphors may be a powerful learning mechanism that could allow children to acquire new information.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 60%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Overall, the present experiment contributes to a growing body of literature suggesting that preschoolers may possess a relatively sophisticated ability to understand and use nonliteral language (Falkum et al, 2017;Pouscoulous & Tomasello, 2020;Zhu, 2021;Zhu et al, 2020). Moreover, by demonstrating that preschoolers can make additional inferences from novel metaphors, the present research suggests that metaphors may be a powerful learning mechanism that could allow children to acquire new information.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 60%
“…Indeed, recent work showed that children develop sophisticated relational-reasoning abilities in their preschool years (Christie & Gentner, 2014; Goddu et al, 2020; Hochmann et al, 2017; Kroupin & Carey, 2022) or even earlier (Anderson et al, 2018; Walker et al, 2016; Walker & Gopnik, 2017). Moreover, additional research demonstrated that preschoolers understand other kinds of nonliteral language, such as metonyms (Falkum et al, 2017; Köder & Falkum, 2020; Zhu, 2021) and metaphors (Pouscoulous & Tomasello, 2020; Zhu et al, 2020, in press). For example, 3-year-olds already understand metaphors based on perceptual similarities (e.g., “The bottle with the big belly” to refer to a round bottle over a slender bottle; Pouscoulous & Tomasello, 2020) but not abstract-motion/space relations (e.g., “Time flies by”; Özçaliskan, 2005).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Several studies report that no additional processing cost is required for comprehending systematic metonymy, and late positivity (pragmatic adjustment) is observed only in the processing of circumstantial metonymy (Frisson & Pickering, 2007;Piñango et al, 2017;Weiland, Bambini, & Schumacher, 2014). Recent work further shows that prior experience of particular exemplars is not necessary for children to learn at least one kind of systematic metonymy: PRODUCER FOR PRODUCT as in 'I read Shakespeare' (Zhu, 2021).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In many natural languages, moderate-to-high-frequency words carry multiple related but distinct meanings (Apresjan, 1974;Copestake & Briscoe, 1995;Nerlich et al, 2003). These kinds of flexible word uses can take different forms, such as morphological conversions (e.g., noun-verb conversions; "I bought a hammer"; "I can hammer the nail"; Srinivasan et al, 2017), metaphors (e.g., space-time metaphors; "This is a long road"; "He took a long time"; Starr & Srinivasan, 2018, 2021, and metonyms (e.g., producer-product metonyms; "Raymond Carver wrote a poem"; "I'm reading Carver"; Littlemore, 2015). These kinds of semantic generalizations not only explain the multiple senses of existing words, but can also facilitate the creation of new word meanings in a lexicon (Srinivasan et al, 2019).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Moreover, while previous research suggests that children struggle to understand non-literal language (e.g., Winner et al, 1976Winner et al, , 1980, more recent work shows that preschoolers can already produce and understand various kinds of non-literal language, such as metaphors and metonyms (Falkum et al, 2017;Pouscoulous & Tomasello, 2020;Zhu, 2021). The current research contributes to this growing body of literature on children's early-emerging capacity to use non-literal language, by showing that children understand novel metonyms that are not conventionally expressed in their everyday language.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%