1998
DOI: 10.1006/ceps.1997.0959
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Learning from Worked-Out Examples: The Effects of Example Variability and Elicited Self-Explanations

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

5
191
1
7

Year Published

2004
2004
2016
2016

Publication Types

Select...
8
1
1

Relationship

1
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 280 publications
(204 citation statements)
references
References 26 publications
5
191
1
7
Order By: Relevance
“…Subsequent studies have shown that prompting for such self-explanations can lead to improved learning outcomes in numerous domains including arithmetic (Calin-Jageman & Ratner, 2005;Rittle-Johnson, 2006;Siegler, 2002), geometry (Aleven & Koedinger, 2002;Wong, Lawson & Keeves, 2003), interest calculations (Renkl, Stark, Gruber & Mandel, 1998), LISP programming (Bielaczyc, Pirolli, & Brown, 1995), argumentation (Schworm & Renkl, 2007), Piagetian number conservation (Siegler, 1995), probability calculation (Große & Renkl, 2003), biology text comprehension (Chi, DeLeeuw, Chiu, & LaVancher, 1994), and balancing beam problems (Pine & Messer, 2000). Moreover, these self-explanation effects have been demonstrated across a wide range of age cohorts, from 5-year-old students (CalinJageman & Ratner, 2005) to adult bank apprentices (Renkl et al, 1998). Perhaps most impressive is that prompting for self-explanation also promotes transfer in many of these domains, even though participants rarely receive feedback on the quality of their explanations (e.g.…”
Section: The Self-explanation Effectmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Subsequent studies have shown that prompting for such self-explanations can lead to improved learning outcomes in numerous domains including arithmetic (Calin-Jageman & Ratner, 2005;Rittle-Johnson, 2006;Siegler, 2002), geometry (Aleven & Koedinger, 2002;Wong, Lawson & Keeves, 2003), interest calculations (Renkl, Stark, Gruber & Mandel, 1998), LISP programming (Bielaczyc, Pirolli, & Brown, 1995), argumentation (Schworm & Renkl, 2007), Piagetian number conservation (Siegler, 1995), probability calculation (Große & Renkl, 2003), biology text comprehension (Chi, DeLeeuw, Chiu, & LaVancher, 1994), and balancing beam problems (Pine & Messer, 2000). Moreover, these self-explanation effects have been demonstrated across a wide range of age cohorts, from 5-year-old students (CalinJageman & Ratner, 2005) to adult bank apprentices (Renkl et al, 1998). Perhaps most impressive is that prompting for self-explanation also promotes transfer in many of these domains, even though participants rarely receive feedback on the quality of their explanations (e.g.…”
Section: The Self-explanation Effectmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Tabelle 1). Da die Aussagekraft von Noten in Bezug auf den tatsächlichen Lernerfolg jedoch umstritten ist (Ruhloff, 1987;Schilmöller, 1992) Lind & Sandmann, 2003, S. 182;Renkl, 1997;Renkl, Stark, Gruber & Mandl, 1998), auch in unserer Untersuchung bestätigt.…”
unclassified
“…High quality argumentation includes at least two central features: First, learners have to provide sound reasons for their claims (Baker, 2003). This (self-)explanation is considered to be a powerful learning activity that fosters learning (Renkl et al, 1998). Furthermore, Stegmann et al (2007a) provided empirical evidence that the quality of single arguments is positively related with the depth of cognitive processing.…”
Section: Argumentative Knowledge Constructionmentioning
confidence: 99%