2012
DOI: 10.1017/s0305000911000481
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Learning foreign labels from a foreign speaker: the role of (limited) exposure to a second language

Abstract: Three- and four-year-olds (N = 144) were introduced to novel labels by an English speaker and a foreign speaker (of Nordish, a made-up language), and were asked to endorse one of the speaker's labels. Monolingual English-speaking children were compared to bilingual children and English-speaking children who were regularly exposed to a language other than English. All children tended to endorse the English speaker's labels when asked 'What do you call this?', but when asked 'What do you call this in Nordish?', … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

3
46
1

Year Published

2012
2012
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
5
1

Relationship

0
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 29 publications
(50 citation statements)
references
References 36 publications
(49 reference statements)
3
46
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Bilingual children have been found to attend and respond more to social partners' communicative cues than have monolingual children (e.g., Brojde, Ahmed, & Colunga, 2012;Yow & Markman, 2011); such greater attention to communicative cues may have helped bilingual children in this study to better attend to information that signaled differences between the two speakers' language backgrounds (e.g., when the bilingual speaker spoke in Japanese). In addition, bilingual children's greater understanding of others' language knowledge (e.g., Akhtar et al, 2012;Byers-Heinlein et al, 2014;Diesendruck, 2005) may have also allowed the bilingual children in this study to better differentiate the two speakers' language backgrounds. That is, bilingual children's greater attention to social partners and enhanced metalinguistic awareness may have allowed bilingual children to differentiate the speakers, identify with whom they shared community membership, and form a preference for whom to imitate.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 4 more Smart Citations
“…Bilingual children have been found to attend and respond more to social partners' communicative cues than have monolingual children (e.g., Brojde, Ahmed, & Colunga, 2012;Yow & Markman, 2011); such greater attention to communicative cues may have helped bilingual children in this study to better attend to information that signaled differences between the two speakers' language backgrounds (e.g., when the bilingual speaker spoke in Japanese). In addition, bilingual children's greater understanding of others' language knowledge (e.g., Akhtar et al, 2012;Byers-Heinlein et al, 2014;Diesendruck, 2005) may have also allowed the bilingual children in this study to better differentiate the two speakers' language backgrounds. That is, bilingual children's greater attention to social partners and enhanced metalinguistic awareness may have allowed bilingual children to differentiate the speakers, identify with whom they shared community membership, and form a preference for whom to imitate.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Monolingual children have indeed been found to have difficulties with metalinguistic awareness, especially in understanding their own and others' language backgrounds (Akhtar et al, 2012;ByersHeinlein et al, 2014). For monolinguals, a speaker's language background may be irrelevant information to consider as long as there is a common language between the monolingual child and speaker.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 3 more Smart Citations