2020
DOI: 10.3389/fcomm.2020.00019
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Ku Waru Clause Chaining and the Acquisition of Complex Syntax

Abstract: How do children learn to understand and use complex syntactic constructions? In English, Diessel (2004) shows that they do so in two different ways. Complex sentences with dependent clauses (e.g., "Peter promised that he would come") develop out of simple sentences that are gradually expanded into multi-clause ones. Complex sentences with coordinate clauses (e.g., "He tried hard, but he failed") develop by integrating two independent sentences into a single two-clause unit. Here we expand on that research by f… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
4
1

Citation Types

1
11
1

Year Published

2020
2020
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
3
2
1

Relationship

0
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 7 publications
(14 citation statements)
references
References 32 publications
1
11
1
Order By: Relevance
“…TO’s three earliest spontaneous two-clause chain productions include one wholly non-conventionalized clause chain, one Continuous aspect construction, and one clause chain using the common pairing of ‘taking it up’ and ‘go.’ Further, TO’s medial verb types counts ( Table 1 ) show no indication that she relies on a very small number of lexical verbs for her chains. Findings here therefore diverge slightly from those for Ku Waru by Rumsey et al (2020) in that Nungon-speaking children’s earliest clause chains are a mix of more and less conventionalized lexical combinations, and there is no indication for Nungon that early chains should be analyzed as involving a single action, rather than two (If this were the case, findings for Nungon clause chain development could be compared to those for early clausal subordination in languages like English and German: Diessel, 2004 ; Brandt et al, 2008 ).…”
Section: Discussioncontrasting
confidence: 90%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…TO’s three earliest spontaneous two-clause chain productions include one wholly non-conventionalized clause chain, one Continuous aspect construction, and one clause chain using the common pairing of ‘taking it up’ and ‘go.’ Further, TO’s medial verb types counts ( Table 1 ) show no indication that she relies on a very small number of lexical verbs for her chains. Findings here therefore diverge slightly from those for Ku Waru by Rumsey et al (2020) in that Nungon-speaking children’s earliest clause chains are a mix of more and less conventionalized lexical combinations, and there is no indication for Nungon that early chains should be analyzed as involving a single action, rather than two (If this were the case, findings for Nungon clause chain development could be compared to those for early clausal subordination in languages like English and German: Diessel, 2004 ; Brandt et al, 2008 ).…”
Section: Discussioncontrasting
confidence: 90%
“…Research into child acquisition of complex sentences has rarely examined the development of sentences comprising more than two clauses. The Nungon data here accords with the Ku Waru data in Rumsey et al (2020) , in that children acquiring these two Papuan languages clearly produce only two-clause chains for a period before beginning to produce chains of more than two clauses. Thus, at least for clause chains in these two languages, children can be said to go through a ‘two-clause stage,’ after which their clause chains expand in length to three clauses (for the Ku Waru children and TO), or more (for Niumen, whose two-clause stage is followed by a stage in which he produces chains of 3–5 clauses).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 85%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…It is an intriguing question for further research whether these differences relate to differences between the languages or between the methods used to investigate them. Some researchers have suggested that clause chains and other similar syntactic constructions may have been exoticized in the literature with a focus on less typical examples [29], [30]. It is possible that impressionistic studies may have been influenced by this bias.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The marked developmental delay in production of different-subject clause chains by children acquiring Ku Waru and Pitjantjatjara can be attributed in large part to the very low proportions of different-subject clause chains in child-directed adult speech in these languages. In most Ku Waru transcripts, 100% of adult clause chains involve a single subject that is maintained throughout the chain (Rumsey et al, 2020). Pitjantjatjara is similar; all but one adult clause chain token in the sample were same-subject (Defina, 2020).…”
Section: Switch-reference and Topic Continuity Within Chains: Is Co-rmentioning
confidence: 97%