2020
DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01586
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Beyond the Two-Clause Sentence: Acquisition of Clause Chaining in Six Languages

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

0
8
0

Year Published

2020
2020
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
4
1

Relationship

2
3

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 7 publications
(9 citation statements)
references
References 30 publications
0
8
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Another indication of verbal sophistication for children acquiring a clause chaining language like Nungon is the production of two-clause, and three-or-more-clause sentences, or ‘clause chains’ (Sarvasy, 2020; Sarvasy & Choi, 2020). For instance, the sentence in (23) is a clause chain comprising three clauses, each with its own simple or complex verbal predicate.…”
Section: Results: Childrenmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Another indication of verbal sophistication for children acquiring a clause chaining language like Nungon is the production of two-clause, and three-or-more-clause sentences, or ‘clause chains’ (Sarvasy, 2020; Sarvasy & Choi, 2020). For instance, the sentence in (23) is a clause chain comprising three clauses, each with its own simple or complex verbal predicate.…”
Section: Results: Childrenmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Both TO and NN use clause chains much more than other complex sentence types, such as subordinate or coordinate structures (Sarvasy, 2020); clause chain use can be taken as partially representative of multi-clause sentence use. Rumsey et al (2020), Sarvasy (2020), and Sarvasy and Choi (2020) showed that in clause chaining languages, children’s earliest clause chain productions comprise just two clauses, with chains of three or more clauses developing later. Thus, production of chains with three or more clauses can also be taken as a milestone in verbal development for these children.…”
Section: Results: Childrenmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…These include: definite and indefinite articles, relative clauses with relative pronouns, the 'have'-perfect, nominative experiencers, participial passives, negative pronouns and a lack of verbal negation, and verb fronting in polar interrogatives (for full list of 'Standard Average European' see Haspelmath, 2001, on the exceptionality of northwestern European languages, see Cysouw, 2011). This skew means that we know much less about features more common outside of European sprachbund, such as tone (Singh & Fu, 2016), polysynthesis (Kelly, Nordlinger, Wigglesworth, & Blythe, 2014;Mithun, 1989), obviation (Henke, 2021), ergativity (Bavin & Stoll, 2013), and clause chaining (Sarvasy & Choi, 2020), to name but a few. It also limits our understanding of how children might acquire typologically rarer phenomena (e.g., symmetrical 'Philippine-style' voice, see Garcia & Kidd, 2020), which are existence proofs of what is possible in natural language and may be particularly challenging to theories built primarily on very different languages.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Clause chains are frequent in Japanese, Korean, and Turkish, but until very recently (namely, the contributions to Sarvasy & Choi, 2020a), there were few studies devoted to acquisition of clause chains, possibly because researchers into acquisition of these languages had oriented themselves toward existing acquisition literature – itself oriented toward features found in English. In fact, new research into clause chaining in these and other languages points to a potentially universal ‘two-clause stage’ in children’s clause combining (Sarvasy & Choi, 2020b). If researchers had not stepped outside the canon of English-like features studied by acquisition scholars, this stage would have remained unrecognized.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%