2017
DOI: 10.1177/1538192717697591
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Knowledge and Beliefs About Developmental Dyslexia: A Comparison Between Pre-Service and In-Service Peruvian Teachers

Abstract: The purpose of this study was to investigate the knowledge, misconceptions, and knowledge gaps of Peruvian pre-service teachers (PSTs) and in-service teachers (ISTs). To do so, 112 PSTs and 113 ISTs completed the Knowledge and Beliefs About Developmental Dyslexia Scale (KBDDS). Results show that ISTs scored significantly higher than PSTs. Moreover, misconceptions and lack of information were higher in PSTs. The most noteworthy misconceptions were that dyslexia is due to poor visual perception and that letter o… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

0
13
0

Year Published

2021
2021
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
5

Relationship

0
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 10 publications
(18 citation statements)
references
References 32 publications
0
13
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Finally, results for beliefs about dyslexia showed that the vast majority of teachers and leaders did not agree with some common misconceptions people have about dyslexia, except for the misconception that students with dyslexia reverse numbers and letters. It is a prevalent misconception in the media and common among teachers (Echegaray‐Bengoa et al, 2017; Ness & Southall, 2010; Serry & Hammond, 2015; Soriano‐Ferrer et al, 2016; Yin et al, 2020). It is a definite misconception because while the number of reversal errors among people with dyslexia may be higher, the proportion of reversal errors as compared to total errors is no greater.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Finally, results for beliefs about dyslexia showed that the vast majority of teachers and leaders did not agree with some common misconceptions people have about dyslexia, except for the misconception that students with dyslexia reverse numbers and letters. It is a prevalent misconception in the media and common among teachers (Echegaray‐Bengoa et al, 2017; Ness & Southall, 2010; Serry & Hammond, 2015; Soriano‐Ferrer et al, 2016; Yin et al, 2020). It is a definite misconception because while the number of reversal errors among people with dyslexia may be higher, the proportion of reversal errors as compared to total errors is no greater.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The fourth misconception was that students with dyslexia have parents who push for more resources for their children—but this does not seem to be the case (Riddick, 1995). The fifth misconception was that students with dyslexia reverse letters and numbers—even though the research says otherwise (Echegaray‐Bengoa et al, 2017; Knight, 2018; Ness & Southall, 2010; Serry & Hammond, 2015; Soriano‐Ferrer et al, 2016; Yin et al, 2020). When reversals persist through the school grades, it seems to be a result of long periods of not reading very well (Krafnick et al, 2014).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Other studies have found that additional teaching experience is positively associated with a better understanding of dyslexia, but these findings are not consistent across all studies or all types of skills and knowledge (Yin et al, 2020); this is reviewed in more detail by Washburn et al (2011). In a study of preservice and in-service teachers in Spain and Peru, in-service teachers had fewer misconceptions than pre-service teachers, demonstrating the benefit of teaching experience and especially experience working with students with dyslexia, in understanding dyslexia (Echegaray-Bengoa, Soriano-Ferrer, & Joshi, 2017;Soriano-Ferrer et al, 2016).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 95%
“…Subsequent studies using the KBDDS found this instrument to be an internally consistent measure of knowledge of dyslexia. Soriano‐Ferrer, Echegaray‐Bengoa, and Joshi (2016) found the reliability of the total scale to be .84, with subscale scores from .68 to .73, and Echegaray‐Bengoa, Soriano‐Ferrer, and Joshi (2017) found reliability of the total scale to be .81, with subscale scores from .67 to .75. The coefficients for Cronbach's alpha for the individual subscales were lower than that of the total scale due to fewer items on each subscale than on the total instrument (Echegaray‐Bengoa et al, 2017; Soriano‐Ferrer et al, 2016).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%