PsycEXTRA Dataset 1973
DOI: 10.1037/e301492005-001
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Judgment Under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

61
3,345
10
142

Year Published

1988
1988
2016
2016

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 2,462 publications
(3,558 citation statements)
references
References 0 publications
61
3,345
10
142
Order By: Relevance
“…(2) How shall we understand the role of content and structure in reasoning? In the dominant program on probabilistic reasoning, the heuristics and biases program (e.g., Tversky & Kahneman, 1974). the formal structure of a 'These results were taken by many colleagues as a demonstration of reasoning errors in human subjects.…”
Section: Research On Deductive and Probabilistic Reasoning: Some Commmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…(2) How shall we understand the role of content and structure in reasoning? In the dominant program on probabilistic reasoning, the heuristics and biases program (e.g., Tversky & Kahneman, 1974). the formal structure of a 'These results were taken by many colleagues as a demonstration of reasoning errors in human subjects.…”
Section: Research On Deductive and Probabilistic Reasoning: Some Commmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The judge may make this judgment by initially assessing, say, the relative frequency of journalists and then adjusting upward to account for additional people who work as realtors (see Rottenstreich & Tversky, 1997). Because adjustments of this sort are typically insufficient (Slovic & Lichtenstein, 1968;Tversky & Kahneman, 1974), this analysis implies that in class judgments support for disjunctions may be monotonic but subadditive. For example, s(journalist or realtor) Ͼ s( journalist) but s(journalist or realtor) Ͻ s(journalist) ϩ s(realtor).…”
Section: In Terms Of Our Example S(journalist or Realtor) ͻ S(journamentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The fallacy presumably arises because Linda appears more similar to or better matches a feminist bank teller than a bank teller. A reliance on the assessment of similarity as an input to judgment has also been shown to contribute to base-rate neglect and nonregressive prediction (Tversky & Kahneman, 1974;Griffin & Tversky, 1992;Bar-Hillel & Fischhoff, 1981), insensitivity to sample size (Tversky & Kahneman, 1971;Borgida & Nisbett, 1977), illusions of covariation assessment (Jennings, Amabile, & Ross, 1982;Chapman & Chapman, 1967, 1969, and misconceptions of random processes (Kahneman & Tversky, 1972;Gilovich, Vallone, & Tversky, 1985;Wagenaar & Bar-Hillel, 1991).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Specifically, the psychological literature suggests that people rely on various mental strategies or cognitive processes to simplify and evaluate complex risk information (Slovic et al, 1979;Fischhoff et al, 1981;Slovic, 1987). These judgmental rules (frequently termed ''heuristics'') are typically based on past experience and intuition (Tversky and Kahneman, 1982). For example, according to the availability heuristic, events are perceived as more frequent or likely to occur if they are easy to imagine or recall.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%